Search results

Filters

  • Journals
  • Authors
  • Keywords
  • Date
  • Type

Search results

Number of results: 23
items per page: 25 50 75
Sort by:
Download PDF Download RIS Download Bibtex

Abstract

Many studies have investigated the relationship between mindfulness and creativity; however, there are a limited number of studies on the neurological basis of this therapeutic approach using electroencephalogram (EEG). This study aimed at evaluating the effect of mindfulness on improving the creativity of healthy individuals. In this study, 7 healthy subjects (1 male and 6 females) with a mean age of 40.37 years and a standard deviation of 14.52 years received group mindfulness training for 8 weeks. They had no experience of mindfulness training up to that time. Before and after mindfulness training, EEG signal was recorded from all participants in eyes-closed and eyes-open conditions on Fz, C3, C4, and Pz electrodes. After data preprocessing, wavelet coefficients were extracted from each frequency band of EEG signal and evaluated using paired sample t-test and correlation methods. The gamma-band on C3 (t = 2.89, p=0.03) and Pz (t= 2.54, P = 0.04) significantly increased as a result of mindfulness training. Also, significant correlations were found between the anxiety and the gamma band in Pz (r = 0.76, P = 0.04) and Fz (r = 0.75, P = 0.04) channels and between arousal and the gamma band in the Fz channel (r=0.88, P = 0.008). Mindfulness training to promote creativity leads to the increase of gamma bands in the central and parietal regions.
Go to article

Bibliography


Abootalebi, V., Moradi, M. H., & Khalilzadeh, M. A. (2009). A new approach for EEG feature extraction in P300-based lie detection. Computer Methods and Programs in Biomedicine, 94(1), 48–57. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cmpb.2008.10.001
Ademoglu, A., Micheli-Tzanakou, E., & Istefanopulos, Y. (1997). Analysis of pattern reversal visual evoked potentials (PRVEP’S) by spline wavelets. IEEE Transactions on Biomedical Engineering, 44(9), 881–890. https://doi.org/10.1109/10.623057
Aftanas, L. I., & Golocheikine, S. A. (2001). Human anterior and frontal midline theta and lower alpha reflect emotionally positive state and internalized attention: High-resolution EEG investigation of medita-tion. Neuroscience Letters, 310(1), 57–60. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0304-3940(01)02094-8
Baer, R. A. (2003). Mindfulness training as a clinical intervention: A conceptual and empirical review. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, Vol. 10, pp. 125–143. https://doi.org/10.1093/clipsy/bpg015
Beik, M., Taheri, H., Saberi Kakhki, A., & Ghoshuni, M. (2020). Neural Mechanisms of the Contextual Interference Effect and Parameter Similarity on Motor Learning in Older Adults: An EEG Study. Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience, 12(June), 1–14. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnagi.2020.00173
Berkovich-Ohana, A., Glicksohn, J., & Goldstein, A. (2012). Mind-fulness-induced changes in gamma band activity - Implications for the default mode network, self-reference and attention. Clinical Neurophysiology, 123(4), 700–710. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.clinph.2011.07.048
Bidin, L., Pigaiani, L., Casini, M., Seghini, P., & Cavanna, L. Feasibility of a trial with Tibetan Singing Bowls, and suggested benefits in metastatic cancer patients. A pilot study in an Italian Oncology Unit. , 8 European Journal of Integrative Medicine § (2016).
Cahn, R. B., Polich, J., Cahn, B. R., & Polich, J. (2013). Meditation states and traits: EEG, ERP, and neuroimaging studies. TL - 1. Psychology of Consciousness: Theory, Research, and Practice, 1 VN-re(S), 48. https://doi.org/10.1037/2326-5523.1.s.48
Crivelli, D., Fronda, G., Venturella, I., & Balconi, M. (2019). Supporting Mindfulness Practices with Brain-Sensing Devices. Cognitive and Electrophysiological Evidences. Mindfulness, 10(2), 301–311. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12671-018-0975-3
Dehghani, S., Amini, K., Shakibazade, E., Faghihzade, S., & Hashem Zade, M. (2015). Effects Of Two Heart Meditation Exercise On Anxiety Among Patients Undergoing Hemodialysis. Preventive Care in Nursing & Midwifery Journal, 4(2), 56–65.
Delorme, A., & Makeig, S. (2004). EEGLAB: An open source toolbox for analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics including independent component analysis. Journal of Neuroscience Methods, 134(1), 9–21. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jneumeth.2003.10.009
Demiralp, T., Yordanova, J., & Kolev, V. (1999). Demiralp99b. 145, 1– 17. Retrieved from papers3://publication/uuid/E9AB3BFE-6460-410B-B907-E3A4ECD94526
Ghoshuni, M., Firoozabadi, M., Khalilzadeh, M., & Hashemi Golpaye-Gani, S. M. R. (2013). Variation of Wavelet Entropy in Electro-encephalogram Signal during Neurofeedback Training. Complexity, 18. https://doi.org/10.1002/cplx.21423
Ivanovski, B., & Malhi, G. S. (2007). The psychological and neurophysiological concomitants of mindfulness forms of medita-tion. Acta Neuropsychiatrica, 19(2), 76–91. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1601-5215.2007.00175.x
Kabat Zinn, J. (1990). Full catastrophe living: Using the wisdom of your body and mind to face stress, pain, and illness/ (Delta trad). Retrieved from https://library.villanova.edu/Find/Record/1428324
Kelly, B. D. (2008). Meditation, mindfulness and mental health. Irish Journal of Psychological Medicine, Vol. 25, pp. 3–4. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0790966700010752
Lomas, T., Ivtzan, I., & Fu, C. H. Y. (2015). A systematic review of the neurophysiology of mindfulness on EEG oscillations. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 57, 401–410. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2015.09.018
Luft, C. D. B., Zioga, I., Banissy, M. J., & Bhattacharya, J. (2019). Spontaneous visual imagery during meditation for creating visual art: An EEG and brain stimulation case study. Frontiers in Psychology, 10(FEB), 1–14. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00210
Michael Murphy, Steven Donovan, E. T. (1997). The physical and psychological effects of meditation: A Review of Contemporary Research. The Physical and Psychological Effects of Meditation: A Review of Contemporary Research With a Comprehensive Bibliography, 1931-1996, 1–23. Retrieved from http://noetic.org/ sites/default/files/uploads/files/Meditation_Intro.pdf
Oh, Y., Chesebrough, C., Erickson, B., Zhang, F., & Kounios, J. (2020). An insight-related neural reward signal. NeuroImage, 214(August 2019), 116757. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.116757
Pour Afrouz, A. Rajai, A. (2018). The construction and psychometric standardization of the irritability questionnaire is a psychoanalysis of Islamic Azad University (pp. 1–13). pp. 1–13. https://doi.org/EPCONF06_132
Ramalingam, V., Cheng, K. S., Sidhu, M. S., & Foong, L. P. (2019). A pilot study: Neurophysiological study on the effect of chronic ankle pain intervene with video assisted mindful deep breathing. 2018 IEEE EMBS Conference on Biomedical Engineering and Sciences, IECBES 2018 - Proceedings, 388–393. https://doi.org/10.1109/IECBES.2018.08626731
Ratcliff, C. G., Prinsloo, S., Chaoul, A., Zepeda, S. G., Cannon, R., Spelman, A., … Cohen, L. (2019). A Randomized Controlled Trial of Brief Mindfulness Meditation for Women Undergoing Stereotactic Breast Biopsy. Journal of the American College of Radiology, 16(5), 691–699. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jacr.2018.09.009
Rokke, P., & Robinson, M. (2006). Book review. Clinical Psychology Review, 26(5), 654–655. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2006.03.001
Rosen, D. S., Oh, Y., Erickson, B., Zhang, F. (Zoe), Kim, Y. E., & Kounios, J. (2020). Dual-process contributions to creativity in jazz improvisations: An SPM-EEG study. NeuroImage, 213(February), 116632. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.116632
Rosso, O. A., Blanco, S., Yordanova, J., Kolev, V., Figliola, A., Schürmann, M., & Ba ar, E. (2001). Wavelet entropy: A new tool for analysis of short duration brain electrical signals. Journal of Neuroscience Methods, 105(1), 65–75. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0165-0270(00)00356-3
Rubia, K. (2009). The neurobiology of Meditation and its clinical effectiveness in psychiatric disorders. Biological Psychology, Vol. 82, pp. 1–11. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2009.04.003
Sairamya, N. J., Premkumar, M. J., George, S. T., & Subathra, M. S. P. (2019). Performance Evaluation of Discrete Wavelet Transform, and Wavelet Packet Decomposition for Automated Focal and Generalized Epileptic Seizure Detection. IETE Journal of Research, 0(0), 1– 21. https://doi.org/10.1080/03772063.2019.1568206
Sibalis, A., Milligan, K., Pun, C., McKeough, T., Schmidt, L. A., & Segalowitz, S. J. (2019). An EEG Investigation of the Attention- Related Impact of Mindfulness Training in Youth With ADHD: Outcomes and Methodological Considerations. Journal of Attention Disorders, 23(7), 733–743. https://doi.org/10.1177/1087054717719535
Stevens, C. E., & Zabelina, D. L. (2019). Creativity comes in waves: an EEG-focused exploration of the creative brain. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 27, 154–162. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cobe-ha.2019.02.003
Taren, A. A., Gianaros, P. J., Greco, C. M., Lindsay, E. K., Fairgrieve, A., Brown, K. W., … Creswell, J. D. (2017). Mindfulness Meditation Training and Executive Control Network Resting State Functional Connectivity: A Randomized Controlled Trial. Psychosomatic Medicine, 79(6), 674–683. https://doi.org/10.1097/PSY.0000000000000466
Tarrant, J., Viczko, J., & Cope, H. (2018). Virtual reality for anxiety reduction demonstrated by quantitative EEG: A pilot study. Frontiers in Psychology, 9(JUL). https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01280
Thought Technology Ltd. (2016). FlexComp System with/ BioGraph Infiniti Software – T7555M. Thoughttechnology.Com. Retrieved from http://thoughttechnology.com/index.php/flexcomp-system-with-biograph-infiniti-software-t7555m.html
Wong, K. F., Teng, J., Chee, M. W. L., Doshi, K., & Lim, J. (2018). Positive effects of mindfulness-based training on energy maintenance and the EEG correlates of sustained attention in a cohort of nurses. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience,
Go to article

Authors and Affiliations

Mahdieh Naderan
1
Majid Ghoshuni
1
ORCID: ORCID
Elham Pour Afrouz
2

  1. Department of Biomedical Engineering, Mashhad Branch, Islamic Azad University, Mashhad, Iran
  2. Institute for Cognitive Science Studies, Tehran, Iran
Download PDF Download RIS Download Bibtex

Abstract

The major aim of the research is to analyse the type and complexity of emotions which adolescent musicians experience before giving a solo music performance. Another aim is to explore the function of these emotions for performance quality. Just before a school concert, students filled out The UWIST Mood Adjective Checklist (UMACL). Right after the performance, both the performing students and competent referees used The Performance Evaluation Scale. The results show that musicians’ pre -performance emotional state is dominated by ambivalent emotions of hope and sadness, as well as joy and anxiety. As a result of a cluster analysis, six clusters were obtained which defined emotional states before the performance: high music performance anxiety, moderate music performance anxiety, calm, mixed emotions, joy with background fatigue, and excitement. The findings show the functional significance of positive emotions and mixed emotions for performance quality.
Go to article

Authors and Affiliations

Julia Kaleńska-Rodzaj
Download PDF Download RIS Download Bibtex

Abstract

Although the emotions and learning based on emotional reaction are individual-specific, the main features are consistent among all people. Depending on the emotional states of the persons, various physical and physiological changes can be observed in pulse and breathing, blood flow velocity, hormonal balance, sound properties, face expression and hand movements. The diversity, size and grade of these changes are shaped by different emotional states. Acoustic analysis, which is an objective evaluation method, is used to determine the emotional state of people’s voice characteristics. In this study, the reflection of anxiety disorder in people’s voices was investigated through acoustic parameters. The study is a case-control study in cross-sectional quality. Voice recordings were obtained from healthy people and patients. With acoustic analysis, 122 acoustic parameters were obtained from these voice recordings. The relation of these parameters to anxious state was investigated statistically. According to the results obtained, 42 acoustic parameters are variable in the anxious state. In the anxious state, the subglottic pressure increases and the vocalization of the vowels decreases. The MFCC parameter, which changes in the anxious state, indicates that people can perceive this situation while listening to the speech. It has also been shown that text reading is also effective in triggering the emotions. These findings show that there is a change in the voice in the anxious state and that the acoustic parameters are influenced by the anxious state. For this reason, acoustic analysis can be used as an expert decision support system for the diagnosis of anxiety.

Go to article

Authors and Affiliations

Turgut Özseven
Muharrem Düğenci
Ali Doruk
Hilal İ. Kahraman
Download PDF Download RIS Download Bibtex

Abstract

Background: The concept of negative transgression describes behavior as crossing personal limits in destruction against oneself or other people. Theoretically, personality dimensions have been distinguished that may trigger or weaken negative transgression, e.g. empathy and anxiety.
Aims: The goal of this study was to empirically verify the relationship between negative transgression and empathy and fear in the perpetrators of violence against close relatives. Our hypothesis was that negative transgression would be associated with poor empathy, but with a high level of anxiety.
Methods: The examined persons from the group of male prisoners (N = 50) were recruited in one of the prisons in Poland. The participants were convicted of domestic violence – or intimate partner violence. Their age ranged from 21 to 50 years.
Results: The results obtained confirm the hypotheses established, showing that prisoners have an increased level of negative transgression and anxiety, and a decreased level of empathy.
Conclusions: This first study shows that more research is needed on negative transgression with different personality dimensions. Also in the process of reclamation of prisoners, it is worth developing empathy and dealing with anxiety. Then the number of negative behaviors in close relationships will decrease.
Go to article

Bibliography

1. Arbach-Lucioni, K., Martinez-García, M., & Andrés-Pueyo, A. (2012). Risk factors for violent behavior in prison inmates: A cross-cultural contribution. Criminal Justice and Behavior, 39(9), 1219–1239. DOI: 10.1177/0093854812445875
2. Azevedo, J., Vieira-Coelho, M., Castelo-Branco, M., Coelho, R., & Figueiredo-Braga, M. (2020). Impulsive and premeditated aggression in male offenders with antisocial personality disorder. PLoS One, 15(3), e0229876. DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0229876
3. Bandura, A., Caprara, G. V., & Regalia, C. (2001). Sociocognitive self- regulatory mechanisms governing transgressive behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80(1), 125-135. DOI: 10.1037/0022-3514.80.1.125
4. Bartkowicz, Z. (1996). Pomoc terapeutyczna nieletnim agresorom i ofiarom agresji w zakładach resocjalizacyjnych [Therapeutic help for minor aggressors and victims of aggression in resocialization facilities]. Lublin: Uniwersytet Marii Curie Skłodowskiej.
5. Batanova, M. D., & Loukas, A. (2011). Social anxiety and aggression in early adolescents: Examining the moderating roles of empathic concern and perspective taking. Journal Youth Adolescence, 40, 1534-1543. DOI: 10.1007/s10964-011-9634-x
6. Batson, C. D., Fultz, J., & Schoenrade, P. A. (1987). Distress and empathy: Two qualitatively distinct vicarious emotions with different motivational consequences. Journal of Personality, 55, 19-39. DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.1987.tb00426.x
7. Beck, C., Anderson, E., O’Hara, K. L., & Benjamin, A. (2013). Patterns of intimate partner violence in a large, epidemiological sample of divorcing couples. Journal of Family Psychology, 27, 743-753. DOI: 10.1037/a0034182
8. Bedyński K., & Wołowicz M. (1989), Zbiorowe protesty w zakładzie penitencjarnym – próba definicji, klasyfikacji przyczyn i metodologiczne propozycje badań zjawiska . [Collective actions in correctional facilities – an attempt to define and classify root causes and methodological research suggestions]. Przegląd Penitencjarny i Kryminologiczny, 14/15, 58-69.
9. Buss, A. H., & Perry, M. (1992). The Aggression Questionnaire. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63(3), 452-459. DOI: 10.1037/0022-3514.63.3.452
10. Casares-López, M. J., González-Menéndez, A., Torres-Lobo, M., Secades-Villa, R., Fernández-Hermida, J. R., & Álvarez, M. M. (2010). Comparison of the psychopathological and addictive profile of two samples of addicts in treatment: in prison and in a therapeutic community. International Journal of Clinical and Health Psychology, 10, 225-243. DOI: 10.22544/rcps.v38i01.04
11. Davis, M. H. (1983). Measuring individual differences in empathy: Evidence for a multidimensional approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 44(1), 113-126. DOI: 10.1037/0022-3514 .44.1.113
12. Davis, M. H. (1999) . Empatia. O umiejętności współodczuwania. [Empathy. The ability to share feelings]. Gdańsk: Gdańskie Wydawnictwo Psychologiczne.
13. Dodge, K. A., & Coie, J. D. (1987) Social-information-processing factors in reactive and proactive aggression in children's peer groups. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53(6), 1146-1158. DOI: 10.1037/0022-3514.53.6.1146
14. Dolan, M. C., & Fullam, R. S. (2010). Moral/conventional transgression distinction and psychopathy in conduct disordered adolescent offenders. Personality and Individual Differences, 49(8), 995-1000. DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0006699
15. Driessen, M., Schroeder, T., Widman, B., Schonfeld, C., & Schneider, F., (2006). Childhood trauma, psychiatric disorders, and criminal behavior in prisoners in Germany: a comparative study in incarcerated women and men. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry 67, 1486–1492. DOI: 10.4088/jcp.v67n100
16. Fazel, S., Bains, P., & Doll, H. (2006). Substance abuse and dependence in prisoners: A systematic review. Addiction, 101, 181-191. DOI: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2006.01316.x
17. Frączek, A. (1996). Violence and aggression in children and youth: A socio-psychological perspective. European Review, 4(1), 75- 90. DOI: 10.1002/(SICI)1234-981X
18. Fromm, E. (1996). Zdrowe społeczeństwo. [The sane society] Warszawa: Państwowy Instytut Wydawniczy.
19. Gini, G. (2006). Social cognition and moral cognition in bullying: What’s wrong? Aggressive Behavior, 32(6), 528-539. DOI: 10.1002/ ab.20153
20. González Collado, C., Iglesias Garcia, M. T., & Sabín Fernández C. (2019). Inteligencia emocional en personas reclusas con discapacidad intelectual. Siglo Cero 50(1), 7. DOI: 10.14201/scero2019501725
21. Gordon, G, Dalton, S., Kolbert, J.B., Kanyongo, G. J., & Crothers, L. M. (2014). Proactive and reactive aggression and cognitive and affective empathy among students in middle childhood, International Journal of School and Cognitive Psychology, 1(2). DOI: 10.4172/2469- 9837.1000105.
22. Hoffman, M. (1975). Sex differences in moral internalization and values. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 32(4), 720-729. DOI: 10.1037/0022-3514.32.4.720
23. Jolliffe, D., & Farrington, D. P. (2004). Empathy and offending: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Aggression and Violent Behavior, 9, 441–476. DOI: 10.1016/j.avb.2003.03.001
24. Jolliffe, D., & Farrington, D. P. (2011). Is low empathy related to bullying after controlling for individual and social background variables? Journal of Adolescence, 34, 1-13. DOI: 10.1016/j.adolescence.2010.02.001
25. Kasari, C., Freeman, S. F. N., & Bass, W. (2003). Empathy and response to distress in children with Down Syndrome. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 44(3), 424-431. DOI: 10.1111/1469-7610.00132
26. Kepiński, A. (1977). Lęk. [Anxiety]. Warszawa: Państwowy Zakład Wydawnictw Lekarskich.
27. Kozielecki, J. (1987). The role of hubristic motivation in transgressive behavior. New Ideas in Psychology, 5(3), 361-383. DOI: 10.1016/0732-118X(87)90006-7
28. Kozielecki, J. (2001). Psychotransgresjonizm. [Psychotransgressionism] Warszawa: Żak.
29. Kubak, F. A., & Salekin, R. T. (2009). Psychopathy and anxiety in children and adolescents: New insights on developmental pathways to offending. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavior Assessment, 31, 271-284. DOI: 10.1007/s10862-009-9144-2
30. Lauterbach, O., & Hosser, D. (2007). Assessing empathy in prisoners – A shortened version of the Interpersonal Reactivity Index. Swiss Journal of Psychology, 66(2), 91-101. DOI: 10.1024/1421-0185.66.2.91
31. Lewicka, A. (2010). Pomiar poziomu empatii emocjonalno-poznawczej Indeksem Reaktywności Interpersonalnej M. H. Davisa [Measurement of the level of emotional and cognitive empathy with the Interpersonal Reactivity Index of M. H. Davis’s]. In: Wojnarska, A. (ed.), Diagnostyka resocjalizacyjna – wybrane zagadnienia [Resocialization diagnostics – selected issues] (pp. 91-105). Lublin: Uniwersytet Marii Curie Skłodowskiej.
32. Lewicka, A., & Lisiecka, M. (2010). Poziom empatii osób odbywających karę pozbawienia wolności [The level of empathy of persons serving the penalty of deprivation of liberty]. In: Z. Jasiński, & D., Widelak (ed.), W poszukiwaniu optymalnego modelu więzienia resocjalizującego [In search of the optimal model of a social rehabilitation prison] (pp. 67-77). Opole: Wydawnictwo Uniwersytetu Opolskiego.
33. Lewicka-Zelent, A., & Huczuk, S. (2016). Empatia a doświadczanie i stosowanie agresji wśród młodzieży [Empathy and experiencing and applying aggression among young people]. Resocjalizacja Polska, 11, 117-132. DOI: 10.22432/pjsr.2016.11.09
34. Loinaz, I., Ortiz-Tallo, M., & Ferragut, M. (2012). MCMI-III Grossman personality facets among partner-violent men in prison. International Journal of Clinical and Health Psychology, 12(3), 389-404. DOI: 10.1080/24732850.2019.1633850
35. Loudin, J. L., Loukas, A., & Robinson, S. (2003). Relation aggression in college students: Examining the roles of social anxiety and empathy. Aggressive Behavior, 29, 430-439. DOI: 10.1002/ab.10039
36. Majchrzyk, Z. (1995). Motywacje zabójczyń. Alkohol i przemoc w rodzinie. [Motives of female killers. Alcohol and domestic abuse]. Warszawa: Państwowa Agencja Rozwiązywania Problemów Alkoholowych.
37. Majchrzyk, Z. (2008). Zabójczynie i zabójcy. [Female and male killers]. Warszawa: Uniwersytet Kardynała Stefana Wyszyńskiego.
38. Martin, S., Zabala, C., Del-Monte, J., Graziani, P., Aizpurua, E., Barry, T. J., & Ricarte, J. (2019). Examining the relationships between impulsivity, aggression, and recidivism for prisoners with antisocial personality disorder. Aggression and Violent Behavior, 49, 101314. DOI: 10.1016/j.avb.2019.07.009
39. McCullough, M. E., Fincham, F. D., & Tsang, J. (2003). Forgiveness, forbearance and time: The temporal unfolding of transgression- related interpersonal motivations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84(3), 540-557. DOI: 10.1037/0022-3514.84.3.540
40. Mehrabian, A., & Epstein, N. (1972), A measure of emotional empathy. Journal of Personality, 40(4), 525-543. DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.1972.tb00078.x
41. Metts, S. (1994). Relational transgressions. In: W. R. Cupach, & B. H. Spitzberg (Eds.), The dark side of interpersonal communication (pp. 217-241). Hillsdale, New Jork: Lawrence Erlbaum.
42. Miller, C. H., Burgoon, J. K., & Hall, J. R. (2007). The effects of implicit theories of moral character on affective reactions to moral transgressions. Social Cognition, 25(6), 819-832. DOI: 10.1521/soco.2007.25.6.819. 43. Ostrowska, K. (2014). Ukryty lęk jako źródło zaburzeń w społecznym funkcjonowaniu jednostki [Hidden anxiety as a source of disorders in an individual’s social functioning]. Profilaktyka Społeczna i Resocjalizacja. 23, 141–149.
44. Ruiz-Hernandez, J. A., Garcia-Jimenez, J. J., Llor-Esteban, B., & Godoy- Fernandez, C. (2015). Risk factors for intimate partner violence in prison inmates. The European Journal of Psychology Applied to Legal Context [online]. 7, (1), 41-49. DOI: 10.1016/j.ejpal.2014.11.003.
45. Sarchiapone, M., Carli, V., Cuomo, C., Marchetti, M., & Roy, A. (2009). Association between childhood trauma and aggression in male prisoners. Psychiatry Research, 165(1-2), 187-192. DOI: 10.1016/j. psychres.2008.04.026
46. Seidel, E. M., Pfabigan, D. M., Keckeis, K., Wucherer, A. M., Jahn, T., Lamm, C., & Derntl, B. (2013). Empathic competencies in violent offenders. Psychiatry Research, 210 (3), 1168-1175. DOI: 10.1016/j. psychres.2013.08.027
47. Ślaski, S. (2012). Motywacyjno-osobowościowe wyznaczniki zachowań transgresyjnych i ochronnych. [Motivational and personal determi-nants of transgressive and defensive behaviours]. Warszawa: Uniwersytet Kardynała Stefana Wyszyńskiego.
48. Ślaski, S. (2013). Lista Zachowań Alternatywnych i Negatywnych. [List of Alternative and Negative Behaviours]. Unpublished copy.
49. Sosnowski, T., Wrześniewski, K., Jaworowska, A., & Fecenec, D. (2011). Inwentarz Stanu i Cechy Lęku STAI. Polska adaptacja STAI. [STAI, the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory. Polish adaptation of STAI.] Fourth extended edition. Warszawa: Pracownia Testów Psychologicznych.
50. Sutton, J., Smith, P. K., & Swettenham, J. (1999). Bullying and ‘theory of mind’: A critique of the ‘social skills deficit’ approach to anti-social behavior. Social Development, 8, 117-127. DOI: 10.1111/1467- 9507.00083
51. Vachon, D. D., Lynam, D. R., & Johnson, J. A. (2014). The (non) relation between empathy and aggression: Surprising results: From a meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin. 140(3), 751-773. DOI: 10.1037/a0035236
52. Værøy, H., Western, E., & Andersson, A. (2016). The link between facets of impulsivity and aggression in extremely violent prisoners. Open Journal of Psychiatry, 6, 86-94. DOI: 10.4236/ojpsych.2016.6101
53. Węgliński, A. (1989). Trafność Kwestionariusza Rozumienia Empatycznego Innych Ludzi (KRE) [Validity of the Empathic Understanding Questionnaire of other people (EUQ)]. In: R. Ł., Drwal (Ed.), Techniki kwestionariuszowe w diagnostyce psychologicznej [Questionnaire techniques in psychological diagnostics] (pp. 63-80). Lublin: Uniwersytet Marii Curie Skłodowskiej.
54. Wojciszke B. (2005). Affective concomitants of information on morality and competence. European Psychologist, 1, 60-70. DOI: 10.1027/1016-9040.10.1.60
55. Wrocławska, E., Rowiński, T. (2008). Kwestionarisz IRI M. Davisa – nowe opracowanie. [IRI questionnaire by M. Davis’s – a new study]. Niepublikowany maszynopis [Unpublished text].
56. Yochelson, S. & Samenow, S. E. (1982). The criminal personality. Volume I: A profile for change. Lanham: Rowman & Littlefield Publishers.
Go to article

Authors and Affiliations

Sławomir Ślaski
1

  1. Cardinal Stefan Wyszyński University
Download PDF Download RIS Download Bibtex

Abstract

The death of a person, particularly my own death, is the most momentous occasion that happens in a lifetime. It seems to be an inevitable end of any possible experience, ceasing any relationship, the end of memories and hopes. It evokes various reactions in the living, just to mention some as: fascination, fear, stress, consent, willingness to familiarization. Each of them may be analysed, while each one shows also the death in a different aspect. In the proposed article, the author indicates another reaction - that is experiencing anxiety. It appears that this is the key experience, both when I am thinking about it as something that may afflict me at any moment, as well as when I become aware that there is a possibility of exit of the loved ones or just a popular person. Some texts by Joseph Ratzinger have inspired me to carry out such analyses.

Go to article

Authors and Affiliations

Ks. Mirosław Pawliszyn CssR
Download PDF Download RIS Download Bibtex

Abstract

The main objective of this study was to analyse the psychometric properties of the Polish adaptation of the Kenny Music Performance Anxiety Inventory – Revised (K -MPAI -R, Kenny, 2009) modified as the Kenny Performance Anxiety Inventory (K -PAI) for a general population of individuals with experience in public performance in fields other than music. Another aim was to test the factor structure of K -PAI on a Polish sample. We analysed the relationship between the scores on K -PAI and general anxiety, depression, attentional control, the scores on the Behavioural Inhibition Scale (BIS) and the Behavioural Activation Scale (BAS) and reward susceptibility. Participants (N = 586) completed the questionnaires in a wider online study. The scores on K -PAI revealed a moderate to strong positive association with different measures of anxiety, trait -anxiety in particular, and negative associations with attentional control and susceptibility to reward. K -PAI scores were strongly associated with depression, but displayed no relationship with the BAS or any of its sub -dimensions. These results generally replicated those obtained on the K -MPAI -R with Australian and Peruvian musicians, indicating the cross -cultural validity of the K -MPAI -R and K -PAI. It is suggested that performance anxiety develops on the basis of the biological predispositions and early negative experiences in performance contexts.
Go to article

Authors and Affiliations

Joanna Kantor-Martynuska
Dianna T. Kenny
Download PDF Download RIS Download Bibtex

Abstract

O b j e c t i v e s: The aim of study was to investigate the association between anxiety, depression, stress and determinants of quality of life among Iranian students.

M e t h o d s: The questionnaires were completed by 275 students. The random sampling was conducted in two phases, the stratified sampling which some classes were selected among different classes of faculty of health and at the second phase, in each class the number of students who had the requirements to enter in the study were selected randomly. the logistic regression to find out the association between demographic characteristics with the quality of life was run and according to the normality status of the distribution of data the parametric or non-parametric tests were used.

R e s u l t s: In the univariable model, the students that were living in their own homes had the odds of 2.18 times more than the others to have a higher quality of life level (95% CI: 1.07–4.45). In the multi variable model the anxiety and stress were significantly related to the quality of life and for increasing each 1 unit in the amount of anxiety and stress the odds of a better quality of life decreases 0.19 and 0.03 respectively. Even after adjusting for other covariates – in the multivariable model – both anxiety and stress were associated with the quality of life.

C o n c l u s i o n: It is useful for the universities to understand different aspects of the students’ lives which are under the influence of stress, anxiety and depression, and also determining the resources from which they are originated.

Go to article

Authors and Affiliations

Bahram Armoon
Yaser Mokhayeri
Javad Haroni
Mahmood Karimy
Mehdi Noroozi
Download PDF Download RIS Download Bibtex

Abstract

This paper investigates attachment themes in the life history narratives of professional orchestral musicians and their relationship with music performance anxiety (MPA). Narrative accounts derived from open -ended in -depth interviews of ten professional musicians were analysed from an attachment perspective using content and thematic analysis. We hypothesized that the performance setting re -triggers unprocessed feelings related to early attachment experiences, especially when traumatic, and that defensive manoeuvres against their re -emergence into consciousness are activated. The interviews identified early relational trauma as a relevant etiological factor in the MPA -symptomatic of the musicians studied. A case is made for the addition of an attachment -informed life -course model rather than a purely symptomatic approach to understanding and treating severe MPA and other intra -personal psychodynamics of performing musicians.
Go to article

Authors and Affiliations

Dianna T. Kenny
Jeremy Holmes
Download PDF Download RIS Download Bibtex

Abstract

Learned helplessness is often studied as a consequence of repetitive failure in a performance domain and is usually associated with the experience of uncontrollability over future outcomes. The premise of this review article is first to establish initial support towards the notion of learned helplessness seen in the context of sports performance. Furthermore, the role of performance anxiety and maladaptive perfectionism will be introduced to strengthen the idea that thinking traits impact motor performance especially when these traits moderate the effects of consecutive failure experience. Finally the paper will focus on a typical profile of an athlete who would be susceptible to choking under pressure as an outcome of perceived uncontrollability and performance anxiety. Burnout and potential interventions will be discussed later.
Go to article

Authors and Affiliations

Sindhuja Sankaran
Download PDF Download RIS Download Bibtex

Abstract

The study was conducted to identify significant predictors of psychological distress in the group of young Polish adults during COVID-19 epidemic outbreak. The web-based cross-sectional survey was applied to 975 Polish respondents (755 female, 77.44%) aged 18-35 years. They were divided into two age groups: younger (18-25) and older (25-35). All participants completed: General Functioning Questionnaire (GFQ), COVID-19 Risk Perception Scale (C-RPS), State Anxiety Inventory (STAI-S), and General Sense of Threat to Life Scale (GSTLS). The Polish adults aged 18-25 years manifested significantly higher psychological distress, state anxiety, and a sense of threat to life, but they perceived less risk in COVID-19 epidemic compared to older participants. Risk perception and a sense of threat to life were indirectly related to state anxiety and psychological distress. Significant predictors of psychological distress in the group of young adult Poles during the COVID-19 epidemic are: state anxiety, risk perception, and a sense of threat to life, where risk perception and a sense of threat to life mediate the relation between state anxiety and psychological distress.
Go to article

Bibliography


Ahmed, M. Z., Ahmed, O., Aibao, Z., Hanbin, S., Siyu, L., & Ahmad, A. (2020). Epidemic of COVID-19 in China and associated Psychological Problems. Asian Journal of Psychiatry, 51. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ajp.2020.102092
Bradley, B. P., Mogg, K., & Millar, N. H. (2000). Covert and overt orienting of attention to emotional faces in anxiety. Cognition and Emotion, 14(6), 789–808. https://doi:10.1080/02699930050156636
Bareket-Bojmel, L., Shahar, G., & Margalit, M. (2020). COVID-19- Related Economic Anxiety Is As High as Health Anxiety: Findings from the USA, the UK, and Israel. International Journal of Cognitive Therapy. https://doi.org/10.1007/s41811-020-00078-3
Ben-Zur, H., & Zeidner, M. (2009). Threat to life and risk-taking behaviors: A review of empirical findings and explanatory models Personality and Social Psychology Review, 13(2), 109–128. https://doi.org/10.1177/1088868308330104
Boyraz, G., & Legros, D. N. (2020). Coronavirus Disease (COVID-19) and Traumatic Stress: Probable Risk Factors and Correlates of Posttraumatic Stress Disorder. Journal of Loss and Trauma, 25(6-7), 503-522, 1–20. https://doi.org/10.1080/15325024.2020.1763556
Bults, M., Beaujean, D. J. M. A., De Zwart, O., Kok, G., Van Empelen, P., Van Steenbergen, J. E., Richardus, J. H., & Voeten, H. A. C. M. (2011). Perceived risk, anxiety, and behavioural responses of the general public during the early phase of the Influenza A (H1N1) pandemic in the Netherlands: Results of three consecutive online surveys. BMC Public Health, 11(1), 2. https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-2458-11-2
Cao, W., Fang, Z., Hou, G., Han, M., Xu, X., Dong, J., & Zheng, J. (2020). The psychological impact of the COVID-19 epidemic on college students in China. Psychiatry Research, 287. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2020.112934
Casagrande, M., Favieri, F., Tambelli, R., & Forte, G. (2020). The enemy who sealed the world: Effects quarantine due to the COVID-19 on sleep quality, anxiety, and psychological distress in the Italian population. Sleep Medicine, 75, 12–20. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.sleep.2020.05.011
Cho, H., & Lee, J. S. (2015). The influence of self-efficacy, subjective norms, and risk perception on behavioral intentions related to the H1N1 flu pandemic: A comparison between Korea and the US. Asian Journal of Social Psychology, 18(4), 311–324. https://doi.org/10.1111/ajsp.12104
Drapeau, A., Marchand, A., & Beaulieu-Prevost, D. (2012). Epidemiology of Psychological Distress. Mental Illnesses – Understanding, Prediction and Control, Luciano L'Abate, IntechOpen. https://doi.org/10.5772/30872
Favieri, F., Forte, G., Tambelli, R., & Casagrande, M. (2020). The Italians in the time of coronavirus: Psychosocial aspects of unexpected COVID-19 pandemic. SSRN. https://doi.org/10.2139/ssrn.3576804
Favieri. F., Forte, G., Tambelli, R. & Casagrande, M. (2021). The Italians in the Time of Coronavirus: Psychosocial Aspects of the Unexpected COVID-19 Pandemic. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 12. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2021.551924
Forte, G., Favieri, F., Tambelli, R., & Casagrande, M. (2020a). COVID- 19 pandemic in the italian population: Validation of a post-traumatic stress disorder questionnaire and prevalence of PTSD symptomatology. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 17(11), 1–16. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph17114151
Forte, G., Favieri, F., Tambelli, R., & Casagrande, M. (2020b). The Enemy Which Sealed the World: Effects of COVID-19 Diffusion on the Psychological State of the Italian Population. Journal of Clinical Medicine, 9(6), 1802. https://doi.org/10.3390/jcm9061802
Fullana, M. A., Hidalgo-Mazzei, D., Vieta, E., & Radua, J. (2020). Coping behaviors associated with decreased anxiety and depressive symptoms during the COVID-19 pandemic and lockdown. Journal of Affective Disorders, 1(275), 80–81. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2020.06.027
Gambin, M., Sękowski, M., Woźniak-Prus, M., Cudo, A., Hansen, K., Gorgol, J., Huflejt-Łukasik, M., Kmita, G., Kubicka, K., Łyś, A., Maison, D., Oleksy, T., & Wnuk, A. (2020). Uwarunkowania objawów depresji i lęku uogólnionego u dorosłych Polaków w trakcie epidemii COVID-19 – raport z pierwszej fali badania podłużnego, 1–27. http://psych.uw.edu.pl/wp-content/uploads/sites/98/2020/05/ Uwarunkowania_objawow_depresji_leku_w_trakcie_pandemii_raport.pdf (accessed 30 May 2021).
Gerhold, L. (2020). COVID-19 : Risk perception and coping strategies. Results from a survey in Germany. Interdisciplinary Security Research Group, 1–11. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/xmpk4
Hawryluck, L., Gold, W. L., Robinson, S., Pogorski, S., Galea, S., & Styra, R. (2004). SARS control and psychological effects of quarantine, Toronto, Canada. Emerging Infectious Diseases, 10(7), 1206–1212. https://doi.org/10.3201/eid1007.030703
Hayes, A. F., Montoya, A. K., & Rockwood, N. J. (2017). The analysis of mechanisms and their contingencies: PROCESS versus structural equation modelling. Australasian Marketing Journal (AMJ), 25(1), 76-81. https://doi:10.1016/j.ausmj.2017.02.001
Huang, Y., & Zhao, N. (2020). Generalized anxiety disorder, depressive symptoms and sleep quality during COVID-19 outbreak in China: a web-based cross-sectional survey. Psychiatry Research, 288. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2020.112954
Knight, R.G., Waal-Manning, H., & Spears, G.D. (1983). Some norms and reliability data for the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory and the Zung Self-Rating Depression scale. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 22, 245-249. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8260.1983.tb00610.x
Okruszek, Ł., Aniszewska-Stańczuk, A., Piejka, A., Wiśniewska, M., & Żurek, K. (2020). Safe but lonely? Loneliness, mental health symptoms and COVID-19. PsyArXiv. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/9njps
Qiu, J., Shen, B., Zhao, M., Wang, Z., Xie, B., & Xu, Y. (2020). A nationwide survey of psychological distress among Chinese people in the COVID-19 epidemic: Implications and policy recommenda-tions. General Psychiatry, 33(2), 19–21. https://doi.org/10.1136/gpsych-2020-100213
Sosnowski, T., Wrześniewski, K., Jaworowska, A., & Fecenec, D. (2006). Inwentarz Stanu i Cechy Lęku STAI. Warszawa: Wyd. Pracownia Testów Psychologicznych Polskiego Towarzystwa Psychologicznego.
Spielberger, C., Gorsuch, R., Lushene, R., Vagg, P., & Jacobs, G. (1983). Manual for the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.
Styla, R., & Kowalski, J. (2020). Psychometric properties of the General Functioning Questionnaire (GFQ-58) used for screening for symptoms of psychopathology and overall level of functioning. Psychiatria Polska, 54(1), 83–100. https://doi.org/10.12740/PP/99564
Sun, N., Wei, L., Shi, S., Jiao, D., Song, R., Ma, L., Wang, H., Wang, C., Wang, Z., You, Y., Liu, S., & Wang, H. (2020). A qualitative study on the psychological experience of caregivers of COVID-19 patients. American Journal of Infection Control, 48(6), 592–598. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ajic.2020.03.018
Tabri N., Hollingshead, S., & Wohl, M. (2020). Framing COVID-19 as an existential threat predicts anxious arousal and prejudice towards Chinese people. PsyArXiv. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/mpbtr
Wheaton, M., Abramowitz, J., Berman, N., Fabricant, L., & Olatunji, B. (2012). Psychological predictors of anxiety in response to the H1N1 (Swine Flu) pandemic. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 36(3), 210– 218. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10608-011-9353-3
World Health Organization. (2020). WHO Director-General’s opening remarks at the media briefing on COVID-19. https://www.who.int/dg/speeches/detail/who-director-general-s-opening-remarks-at-the-media-briefing-on-covid-19—11-march-2020 (accessed 5 May 2021).
Go to article

Authors and Affiliations

Beata Mirucka
1
Urszula Bielecka
2
Maria Mirucka
3
Natalia Kępińska
4

  1. John Paul II Catholic University of Lublin, Lublin, Poland
  2. University of Bialystok, Białystok, Poland
  3. University of Warsaw, Warsaw, Poland
  4. SWPS University, Warsaw, Poland
Download PDF Download RIS Download Bibtex

Abstract

The misinformation effect is influenced by many mnestic and non-mnestic factors. This article concerns the role of two of them: 1) state anxiety, defined as a situational experience of anxiety; 2) memory distrust, understood as a constant tendency to negatively evaluate one's memory. Both factors are relevant in the situation of being a witness and are believed to have a negative effect on the magnitude of the misinformation effect. In the present research, participants’ state anxiety had an immunizing effect against misinformation. As for memory distrust, no relationship was found between negative evaluation of memory and susceptibility to misinformation. The results confirm the beneficial effect of anxiety on resisting misinformation and demonstrate a greater need for further explorations concerning memory distrust.
Go to article

Bibliography


Ayers, M. S., & Reder, L. M. (1998). A theoretical review of the misinformation effect: Predictions from an activation-based memory model. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 5(1), 1–21. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03209454
Bar-Haim, Y., Lamy, D., Pergamin, L., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., & van Ijzendoorn, M. H. (2007). Threat-related attentional bias in anxious and nonanxious individuals: A meta-analytic study. Psychological Bulletin, 133(1), 1–24. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.133.1.1
Bensi, L., & Giusberti, F. (2007). Trait anxiety and reasoning under uncertainty. Personality and Individual Differences, 43(4), 827–838. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2007.02.007
Blank, H. (1998). Memory states and memory tasks: An integrative framework for eyewitness memory and suggestibility. Memory, 6(5), 481–529. https://doi.org/10.1080/741943086
Boelen, P. A., & Reijntjes, A. (2009). Intolerance of uncertainty and social anxiety. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 23(1), 130–135. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.janxdis.2008.04.007
Brewer, N., & Wells, G. L. (2011). Eyewitness identification. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 20(1), 24–27. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721410389169
Cline, M. E., Herman, J., Shaw, E. R., & Morton, R. D. (1992). Standardization of the Visual Analogue Scale. Nursing Research, 41(6), 378–380. https://doi.org/10.1097/00006199-199211000-00013
Cohen, R. L., & Harnick, M. A. (1980). The susceptibility of child witnesses to suggestion: An empirical study. Law and Human Behavior, 4(3), 201–210. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01040318
Commissaris, C. J. a. M., Ponds, R. W. H. M., & Jolles, J. (1998). Subjective forgetfulness in a normal Dutch population: Possibilities for health education and other interventions. Patient Education and Counseling, 34(1), 25–32. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0738-3991(98)00040-8
Constans, J. I., Penn, D. L., Ihen, G. H., & Hope, D. A. (1999). Interpretive biases for ambiguous stimuli in social anxiety. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 37(7), 643–651. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0005-7967(98)00180-6
Dalton, A. L., & Daneman, M. (2006). Social suggestibility to central and peripheral misinformation. Memory, 14(4), 486–501. https://doi.org/10.1080/09658210500495073
Derakshan, N., & Eysenck, M. W. (1997). Interpretive biases for one’s own behavior and physiology in high-trait-anxious individuals and repressors. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73(4), 816–825. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.73.4.816
Derryberry, D., & Reed, M. A. (2002). Anxiety-related attentional biases and their regulation by attentional control. Journal of abnormal psychology, 111(2), 225–236. https://doi.org/10.1037/0021- 843X.111.2.225
Duncan, E. M., Whitney, P., & Kunen, S. (1982). Integration of visual and verbal information in children’s memories. Child Development, 53(5), 1215–1123. https://doi.org/10.2307/1129008
Eisen, M. L., Gabbert, F., Ying, R., & Williams, J. (2017). „I think he had a tattoo on his neck”: How co-witness discussions about a perpe-trator’s description can affect eyewitness identification decisions. Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition, 6(3), 274– 282. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jarmac.2017.01.009
Eysenck, M. W., & Calvo, M. G. (1992). Anxiety and performance: The processing efficiency theory. Cognition and Emotion, 6(6), 409–434. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699939208409696
Eysenck, M. W., Derakshan, N., Santos, R., & Calvo, M. G. (2007). Anxiety and cognitive performance: Attentional control theory. Emotion, 7(2), 336–353. https://doi.org/10.1037/1528-3542.7.2.336
Frost, P., Ingraham, M., & Wilson, B. (2002). Why misinformation is more likely to be recognised over time: A source monitoring account. Memory, 10(3), 179–185. https://doi.org/10.1080/09658210143000317
Goodwin, K. A., Kukucka, J. P., & Hawks, I. M. (2013). Co‐witness confidence, conformity, and eyewitness memory: An examination of normative and informational social influences. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 27(1), 91–100. https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.2877
Graeff, F. G., Parente, A., Del-Ben, C. M., & Guimarães, F. S. (2003). Pharmacology of human experimental anxiety. Brazilian Journal of Medical and Biological Research, 36(4), 421–432. https://doi.org/10.1590/S0100-879X2003000400003
Gudjonsson, G. H. (2003). The Psychology of Interrogations and Confessions: A Handbook. Chichester: Jon Wiley & Sons.
Gudjonsson, G. H., & Clark, N. K. (1986). Suggestibility in police interrogation: A social psychological model. Social Behaviour, 1(2), 83–104.
Gudjonsson, G. H., & MacKeith, J. A. C. (1982). False confessions: Psychological effects of interrogation. W: A. Trankell (red.), Reconstructing the past: The role of psychologists in criminal trials (p. 253-269). Zuidpoolsingel: Kluwer Law International.
Gudjonsson, G. H., Kopelman, M. D., & MacKeith, J. A. C. (1999). Unreliable admissions to homicide: A case of misdiagnosis of amnesia and misuse of abreaction technique. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 174, 455–459. https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.174.5.455
Gudjonsson, G. H., Sigurdsson, J. F., Sigurdardottir, A. S., Steinthorsson, H., & Sigurdardottir, V. M. (2014). The role of memory distrust in cases of internalised false confession. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 28(3), 336–348. https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.3002
Hayes, A. F. (2018). Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis. A regression-based approach. (second edition) New York: The Guilford Press.
Hoscheidt, S. M., LaBar, K. S., Ryan, L., Jacobs, W. J., & Nadel, L. (2014). Encoding negative events under stress: High subjective arousal is related to accurate emotional memory despite misinforma-tion exposure. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 112, 237– 247. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nlm.2013.09.008
Kuczek, M., Szpitalak, M., & Polczyk, R. (2018). Psychometric properties and correlates of the Polish version of the Squire Subjective Memory Questionnaire (SSMQ). Personality and Individual Differences, 120, 271–275. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2016.05.021
Lindsay, D. S., & Johnson, M. K. (1989). The eyewitness suggestibility effect and memory for source. Memory & Cognition, 17(3), 349–358. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03198473
Loftus, E. F., Miller, D. G., & Burns, H. J. (1978). Semantic integration of verbal information into a visual memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 4(1), 19–31. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.4.1.19
Luna, K., & Martín-Luengo, B. (2012). Improving the Accuracy of Eyewitnesses in the Presence of Misinformation with the Plurality Option. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 26(5), 687–693. https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.2845
MacLeod, C., & Cohen, I. L. (1993). Anxiety and the interpretation of ambiguity: A text comprehension study. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 102(2), 238–247. https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-843X.102.2.238
Mathews, A., May, J., Mogg, K., & Eysenck, M. (1990). Attentional bias in anxiety: Selective search or defective filtering? Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 99(2), 166–173. https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-843X.99.2.166
McEvoy, P. M., & Mahoney, A. E. J. (2012). To be sure, to be sure: Intolerance of uncertainty mediates symptoms of various anxiety disorders and depression. Behavior Therapy, 43(3), 533–545. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beth.2011.02.007
McNair, D. M., Frankenthaler, L. M., Czerlinsky, T., White, T. W., Sasson, S., & Fisher, S. (1982). Simulated public speaking as a model of clinical anxiety. Psychopharmacology, 77(1), 7–10. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00436092
Mogg, K., Mathews, A., & Eysenck, M. (1992). Attentional bias to threat in clinical anxiety states. Cognition and Emotion, 6(2), 149–159. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699939208411064
Nitschke, J. P., Chu, S., Pruessner, J. C., Bartz, J. A., & Sheldon, S. (2019). Post-learning stress reduces the misinformation effect: Effects of psychosocial stress on memory updating. Psychoneuro-endocrinology, 102, 164–171. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psy-neuen.2018.12.008
Pickrell, J. E., McDonald, D.-L., Bernstein, D. M., & Loftus, E. F. (2017). Misinformation effect. W R. F. Pohl (Red.), Cognitive illusions: Intriguing phenomena in thinking, judgment and memory., 2nd ed. (2016-19747-023; s. 406–423). Routledge/Taylor & Francis Group.
Polczyk, R. (2007). Mechanizmy efektu dezinformacji w kontekście zeznań świadka naocznego [Mechanisms of the misinformation effect in the context of witness testimony]. Kraków: Wydawnictwo Uniwersytetu Jagiellońskiego.
Polczyk, R. (2017). The ‘memory’ misinformation effect may not be caused by memory failures : exploring memory states of misinformed subjects. Polish Psychological Bulletin, 48(3), 388-400. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1515/ppb-2017-0045
Ridley, A. M., & Clifford, B. R. (2004). The effects of anxious mood induction on suggestibility to misleading post-event information. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 18(2), 233–244. https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.963
Ridley, A. M., & Clifford, B. R. (2006). Suggestibility and state anxiety: How the two concepts relate in a source identification paradigm. Memory, 14(1), 37–45. https://doi.org/10.1080/09658210444000494
Ridley, A. M., Clifford, B. R., & Keogh, E. (2002). The effects of state anxiety on the suggestibility and accuracy of child eyewitnesses. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 16(5), 547–558. https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.813
Skagerberg, E. M., & Wright, D. B. (2008). The prevalence of co- witnesses and co-witness discussions in real eyewitnesses. Psychol-ogy, Crime & Law, 14(6), 513-521. https://doi.org/10.1080/10683160801948980
Spielberger, C. D., Gorsuch, R. L., Lushene, R. E. (1970). The State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI). Test Manual for Form X. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.
Squire, L. R., Wetzel, C. D., & Slater, P. C. (1979). Memory complaint after electroconvulsive therapy: Assessment with a new self-rating instrument. Biological Psychiatry, 14(5), 791–801.
Tousignant, J. P., Hall, D., & Loftus, E. F. (1986). Discrepancy detection and vulnerability to misleading postevent information. Memory & Cognition, 14(4), 329–338. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03202511
van Bergen, S., Brands, I., Jelicic, M., & Merckelbach, H. (2010). Assessing trait memory distrust: Psychometric properties of the Squire Subjective Memory Questionnaire. Legal and Criminological Psychology, 15(2), 373–384. https://doi.org/10.1348/135532509X471960
van Bergen, S., Jelicic, M., & Merckelbach, H. (2008). Interrogation techniques and memory distrust. Psychology, Crime & Law, 14(5), 425–434. https://doi.org/10.1080/10683160701822533
van Bergen, S., Jelicic, M., & Merckelbach, H. (2009). Are subjective memory problems related to suggestibility, compliance, false memories, and objective memory performance? The American Journal of Psychology, 122(2), 249–257.
VanOss Marin, B., Holmes, D. L., Guth, M., & Kovac, P. (1979). The potential of children as eyewitnesses: A comparison of children and adults on eyewitness tasks. Law and Human Behavior, 3(4), 295– 306. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01039808
Wade, K. A., Green, S. L., & Nash, R. A. (2010). Can fabricated evidence induce false eyewitness testimony?. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 24(7), 899-908. https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.1607
Wilson, E. J., MacLeod, C., Mathews, A., & Rutherford, E. M. (2006). The causal role of interpretive bias in anxiety reactivity. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 115(1), 103–111. https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-843X.115.1.103
Wright, D. B., London, K., & Waechter, M. (2010). Social anxiety moderates memory conformity in adolescents. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 24(7), 1034–1045. https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.1604
Wright, D. B., Memon, A., Skagerberg, E. M., & Gabbert, F. (2009). When eyewitnesses talk. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 18(3), 174–178. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8721.2009.01631.x
Wrześniewski, K., Sosnowski, T. (1996). Inwentarz stanu I cechy lęku (ISCL). Polska adaptacja STAI. [The State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI). Polish adaptaion.] Warszawa: Pracownia Testów Psychologicznych.
Zaragoza, M. S., & Lane, S. M. (1994). Source misattributions and the suggestibility of eyewitness memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 20(4), 934–945. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.20.4.934
Zhu, B., Chen, C., Loftus, E. F., Lin, C., He, Q., Chen, C., Li, H., Xue, G., Lu, Z., & Dong, Q. (2010). Individual differences in false memory from misinformation: Cognitive factors. Memory, 18(5), 543–555. https://doi.org/10.1080/09658211.2010.487051
Zhu, B., Chen, C., Loftus, E. F., Lin, C., He, Q., Chen, C., Li, H., Moyzis, R. K., Lessard, J., & Dong, Q. (2010). Individual differences in false memory from misinformation: Personality characteristics and their interactions with cognitive abilities. Personality and Individual Differences, 48(8), 889–894. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2010 .02.016
Go to article

Authors and Affiliations

Marta Kuczek
1
Malwina Szpitalak
1
ORCID: ORCID
Romuald Polczyk
1

  1. Institute of Psychology, Jagiellonian University, Poland
Download PDF Download RIS Download Bibtex

Abstract

The present study explores the connection between the actual/ideal (A/I) and actual/ought (A/O) self- -discrepancies and negative emotional states such as stress, anxiety and depression. Moreover, it seeks to understand the effects of potentially intervening variables, self-control //and self-consciousness, on the affect-discrepancy relationship. 638 participants (60% female, aged 18-55) participated in the study. They filled out questionnaires measuring actual/ ideal self-discrepancy, actual/ought self-discrepancy, self-control, private/public self-consciousness and psychological distress (depression, anxiety and stress; DAS). The results revealed that both, A/O and A/I self-discrepancies, are positively associated with DAS but do not have a predictive value for them. However, depression, anxiety and stress are significantly predicted by low self-control and high personal self-consciousness. Also, the study confirms that self- -control and self-consciousness moderate affect-discrepancy relationship: self-control is a significant moderator of the relationships between (1) A/I and A/O self-discrepancy and depression and (2) A/I and A/O self-discrepancy and stress. Also, public self-consciousness moderates the relationship between A/O self-discrepancy and stress. In this respect those who have high self-control and high self-consciousness are less likely to experience negative emotional reactions related to the discrepant self-constructs.
Go to article

Bibliography

1.Ashford, K. J., Karageorghis, C. I., & Jackson, R. C. (2005). Modeling the relationship between self-consciousness and competition anxiety. Personality and Individual Differences, 38(4), 903–918. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2004.06.015
2. Barnett, M. D., Moore, J. M., & Harp, A. R. (2017). Who we are and how we feel: Self-discrepancy theory and specific affective states. Personality and Individual Differences, 111, 232–237. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2017.02.024
3. Baumeister, R. F. (1999). The nature and structure of the self: An overview. In R. F. Baumeister (Ed.),The self in social psychology (pp. 1-20). Ann Arbor, MI: Psychology Press. - Google Search. (n.d.).
4. Baumeister, R. F., & Heatherton, T. F. (1996). Self-regulation failure: An overview. Psychological Inquiry, 7(1), 1–15. https://doi.org/10.1207/ s15327965pli0701_1
5. Ben-Artzi, E., & Hamburger, Y. A. (2016). Private Self-Consciousness Subscales: Correlates with Neuroticism, Extraversion, and Self-Discrepancy: Imagination, Cognition and Personality. https://doi.org/10.2190/LL2E-5RFH-45FJ-HXL7
6. Bizman, A., Yinon, Y., & Krotman, S. (2001). Group-Based Emotional Distress: An Extension of Self-Discrepancy Theory. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 27(10), 1291–1300. https://doi.org/10.1177/01461672012710005
7. Boldero, J., & Francis, J. (2000). The relation between self-discrepancies and emotion: The moderating roles of self-guide importance, location relevance, and social self-domain centrality. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78(1), 38–52. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022- 3514.78.1.38
8. Bowlin, S. L., & Baer, R. A. (2012). Relationships between mindfulness, self-control, and psychological functioning. Personality and Individual Differences, 52(3), 411–415. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. paid.2011.10.050
9. Bruch, M. A., Rivet, K. M., & Laurenti, H. J. (2000). Type of self-discrepancy and relationships to components of the tripartite model of emotional distress. Personality and Individual Differences, 29(1), 37–44. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0191-8869(99)00176-2
10. Burke, P. J., & Harrod, M. M. (2016). Too Much of a Good Thing?: Social Psychology Quarterly. https://doi.org/10.1177/ 019027250506800404
11. Calogero, R. M., & Watson, N. (2009). Self-discrepancy and chronic social self-consciousness: Unique and interactive effects of gender and real–ought discrepancy. Personality and Individual Differences, 46(5), 642–647. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2009.01.008
12. Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (1978). Self-focusing effects of dispositional self-consciousness, mirror presence, and audience presence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 36(3), 324–332. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.36.3.324
13. Carver, C. S., Lawrence, J. W., & Scheier, M. F. (1999). Self-Discrepancies and Affect: Incorporating the Role of Feared Selves. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 25(7), 783–792. https:// doi.org/10.1177/0146167299025007002
14. Dickson, J. M., Moberly, N. J., & Huntley, C. D. (2019). Rumination selectively mediates the association between actual-ideal (but not actual-ought) self-discrepancy and anxious and depressive symptoms. Personality and Individual Differences, 149, 94–99. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2019.05.047
15. Duval, S., & Wicklund, R. A. (1972). A theory of objective self awareness. Academic Press.
16. Eichstaedt, J., & Silvia, P. J. (2003). Noticing the self: Implicit assessment of self-focused attention using word recognition latencies. Social Cognition, 21(5), 349–361. https://doi.org/ 10.1521/soco.21.5.349.28686
17. Fromson, P. M. (2006). Self-discrepancies and negative affect: The moderating roles of private and public self-consciousness. Social Behavior and Personality, 34(4), 333–350. https://doi.org/10.2224/ sbp.2006.34.4.333 18. Gillebaart, M. (2018). The ‘Operational’ Definition of Self-Control. Frontiers in Psychology, 9. https://doi.org/10.3389/ fpsyg.2018.01231
19. Gibbons, F. X. (1990). Self-Attention and Behavior: A Review and Theoretical Update. In Advances in Experimental Social Psychology (Vol. 23, pp. 249–303). Elsevier. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0065-2601(08)60321-4
20. Gonnerman, M. E., Parker, C. P., Lavine, H., & Huff, J. (2000). The Relationship between Self-Discrepancies and Affective States: The Moderating Roles of Self-Monitoring and Standpoints on the Self. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 26(7), 810–819. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167200269006
21. Hardin, E. E., & Lakin, J. L. (2009). The Integrated Self-Discrepancy Index: A reliable and valid measure of self-discrepancies. Journal of Personality Assessment, 91(3), 245–253. https://doi.org/10.1080/ 00223890902794291
22. Hayes, A. F. (2013). Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis: A regression-based approach. New York: The Guilford Press.
23. Heppen, J. B., & Ogilvie, D. M. (2003). Predicting affect from global self-discrepancies: The dual role of the undesired self. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 22(4), 347–368. https://doi.org/10.1521/jscp.22.4.347.22898
24. Higgins, E. T., Klein, R., & Strauman, T. (1985). Self-concept discrepancy theory: A psychological model for distinguishing among different aspects of depression and anxiety. Social Cognition, 3(1), 51–76. https://doi.org/10.1521/soco.1985.3.1.51
25. Higgins, E. T. (1987). Self-discrepancy: A theory relating self and affect. Psychological Review, 94(3), 319–340. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033- 295X.94.3.319
26. Higgins, E. T. (1996). The “self digest”: Self-knowledge serving self- regulatory functions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71(6), 1062–1083. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.71.6.1062
27. Hofmann, W., Luhmann, M., Fisher, R. R., Vohs, K. D., & Baumeister, R. F. (2014). Yes, But Are They Happy? Effects of Trait Self-Control on Affective Well-Being and Life Satisfaction. Journal of Personality, 82(4), 265–277. https://doi.org/10.1111/jopy.12050
28. Hull, J. G., Van Treuren, R. R., Ashford, S. J., Propsom, P., & Andrus, B. W. (1988). Self-consciousness and the processing of self-relevant information. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54(3), 452–465. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.54.3.452
29. Ickes, W. J., Wicklund, R. A., & Ferris, C. B. (1973). Objective self awareness and self esteem. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 9(3), 202–219. https://doi.org/10.1016/0022-1031(73)90010-3
30. Kivetz, R., & Keinan, A. (2006). Repenting Hyperopia: An Analysis of Self-Control Regrets. Journal of Consumer Research, 33(2), 273– 282. https://doi.org/10.1086/506308
31. Koole, S. L., Tops, M., Strübin, S., Bouw, J., Schneider, I. K., & Jostmann, N. B. (2014). The ego fixation hypothesis: Involuntary persistence of self-control. In Motivation and its regulation: The control within (pp. 95–112). Psychology Press.
32. Layton, R. L., & Muraven, M. (2014). Self-control linked with restricted emotional extremes. Personality and Individual Differences, 58, 48– 53. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2013.10.004
33. Lovibond, S. H., Lovibond, P. F., & Australia, P. F. of. (1995). Manual for the depression anxiety stress scales. Sydney, N.S.W. : Psychology Foundation of Australia. https://trove.nla.gov.au/version/46688692
34. Mathes, E. W., Lane, D. J., Helmers, B. R., Jamnik, M. R., Hendrickson, M., & Aleshire, B. (2017). The dark side of self-control: High self-control leads to better outcomes when engaging in bad behaviors. Personality and Individual Differences, 105, 326–329. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2016.10.005
35. Milyavskaya, M., & Inzlicht, M. (2017). What’s So Great About Self- -Control? Examining the Importance of Effortful Self-Control and Temptation in Predicting Real-Life Depletion and Goal Attainment. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 8(6), 603–611. https://doi.org/10.1177/1948550616679237
36. Muraven, M., & Baumeister, R. F. (2000). Self-regulation and depletion of limited resources: Does self-control resemble a muscle? Psychological Bulletin, 126(2), 247–259. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033- 2909.126.2.247
37. Petrocelli, J. V., & Smith, E. R. (2005). Who I am, who we are, and why: Links between emotions and causal attributions for self- and group discrepancies. Personality & Social Psychology Bulletin, 31(12), 1628–1642. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167205277390
38. Phillips, A. G., & Silvia, P. J. (2005). Self-awareness and the emotional consequences of self-discrepancies. Personality & Social Psychology Bulletin, 31(5), 703–713. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167204271559
39. Scheier, M. F., & Carver, C. S. (1985). The Self-Consciousness Scale: A revised version for use with general populations. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 15(8), 687–699. https://doi.org/10.1111/ j.1559-1816.1985.tb02268.x
40. Scheier, M. F., & Carver, C. S. (1980). Private and public self-attention, resistance to change, and dissonance reduction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39(3), 390–405. https://doi.org/10.1037/ 0022-3514.39.3.390
41. Scott, L., & O’Hara, M. W. (1993). Self-discrepancies in clinically anxious and depressed university students. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 102(2), 282–287. https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-843X.102.2.282
42. Silvia, P. J., & Gendolla, G. H. E. (2001). On Introspection and Self-Perception: Does Self-Focused Attention Enable Accurate Self-Knowledge?: Review of General Psychology. https://journals.sagepub.com/doi/10.1037/1089-2680.5.3.241
43. Silvia, P. J. (2002). Self-awareness and emotional intensity. Cognition and Emotion, 16(2), 195–216. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699930143000310
44. Silvia, P. J., & Duval, T. S. (2001). Objective self-awareness theory: Recent progress and enduring problems. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 5(3), 230–241. https://doi.org/10.1207/ S15327957PSPR0503_4
45. Stavrova, O., Pronk, T., & Kokkoris, M. D. (2019). Choosing goals that express the true self: A novel mechanism of the effect of self-control on goal attainment. European Journal of Social Psychology, 49(6), 1329–1336. https://doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.2559
46. Stavrova, O., Pronk, T., & Kokkoris, M. D. (2020). Finding meaning in self-control: The effect of self-control on the perception of meaning in life. Self and Identity, 19(2), 201–218. https://doi.org/10.1080/15298868.2018.1558107
47. Stevens, E. N., Holmberg, N. J., Lovejoy, M. C., & Pittman, L. D. (2014). When do self-discrepancies predict negative emotions? Exploring formal operational thought and abstract reasoning skills as moderators. Cognition and Emotion, 28(4), 707–716. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2013.845082
48. Strauman, T. J., & Higgins, E. T. (1988). Self-discrepancies as predictors of vulnerability to distinct syndromes of chronic emotional distress. Journal of Personality, 56(4), 685–707. https://doi.org/10.1111/ j.1467-6494.1988.tb00472.x
49. Swann Jr., W. B. (2012). Self-verification theory. In Handbook of theories of social psychology, Vol. 2 (pp. 23–42). Sage Publications Ltd. https://doi.org/10.4135/9781446249222.n27
50. Tangney, J. P., Baumeister, R. F., & Boone, A. L. (2004). High self-control predicts good adjustment, less pathology, better grades, and interpersonal success. Journal of Personality, 72(2), 271–324. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.0022-3506.2004.00263.x
51. Thomson, W. (2016). Depression, Neuroticism, and the Discrepancy Between Actual and Ideal Self-Perception. Personality and Individual Differences, 88, 219–224. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2015.09.003
52. Tangney, J. P., Niedenthal, P. M., Covert, M. V., & Barlow, D. H. (1998). Are shame and guilt related to distinct self-discrepancies? A test of Higgins’s (1987) hypotheses. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75(1), 256–268. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022- 3514.75.1.256


Go to article

Authors and Affiliations

Maia Mestvirishvili
1
Natia Mestvirishvili
2

  1. Ivane Javakhishvili Tbilisi State University
  2. Caucasus University
Download PDF Download RIS Download Bibtex

Abstract

The issue of math attitude and math anxiety in STEM students has been till now overlooked. However, the issue occurring in many countries is students’ falling out of the STEM education system during their studies. One of the reasons for this problem may be high math anxiety and a negative math attitude among students. The present study fills a gap in knowledge about this phenomenon among STEM students. 371 Polish STEM students filled questionnaires of math attitude (MASA) and math anxiety (MAQA, SIMA, AMAS). The results are as follow: The mean results show that STEM students have a very positive math attitude in affective and cognitive dimensions and a rather positive math attitude in the behavioral area; On average, STEM students feel very weak anxiety related to math problem solving, weak general math anxiety and math learning anxiety, and a moderate level of math testing anxiety; Among STEM students there are those who present a very negative/negative math attitude and very strong/strong math anxiety; Women feel more intense anxiety related to math problem solving, but there is no gender gap in general math anxiety, math learning and math testing anxiety, and in math attitude. The results suggest that math attitude and math anxiety of STEM students should be monitored. Indeed, not all STEM students have a positive math attitude and feel no math anxiety. Moreover, proper interventions are recommended to decrease math anxiety and improve positive math attitude that in turn may prevent the students’ dropping out from STEM studies.
Go to article

Authors and Affiliations

Monika Szczygieł
1
ORCID: ORCID

  1. Pedagogical University of Cracow, Poland
Download PDF Download RIS Download Bibtex

Abstract

Although the usefulness of alternative assessment in second language (L2) classrooms has been extensively recognized by scholars, the use of the various types of alternative assessment in English as a Foreign Language (EFL) contexts has not received adequate attention by L2 practitioners. To contribute to this line of research, the present research sought to examine the impact of a portfolio-based writing instruction on writing performance and writing anxiety of EFL students. To this end, a number of 41 EFL learners were recruited as the participants of this study. They were then randomly divided to an experimental group (N=21) and a control group (N=20). The participants in the experimental group received portfolio-based writing instruction, whereas the control group received the regular writing instruction with no archiving of students’ drafts in portfolios. Timed-writing tasks and the Second Language Writing Anxiety Inventory (SLWAI) were employed to collect the data. The results obtained from ANCOVA analysis revealed that the portfolio-based writing instruction aided the participants in improving their writing performance more than the control group. Moreover, it was found that the use of portfolios significantly reduced the L2 writing anxiety of the participants while the traditional writing instruction did not have any significant impact on L2 writing anxiety of the control group. The pedagogical implications for portfolio-based writing instruction are discussed finally.

Go to article

Authors and Affiliations

Jalil Fathi
Ali Derakhshan
Maryam Safdari
Download PDF Download RIS Download Bibtex

Abstract

This is an obituary of Harold Bloom and a brief review of his two books: The Western Canon and How to Read and Why, which were recently translated into Polish. It also outlines Bloom’s unique conception of literature and his praise of solitary reading.

Go to article

Authors and Affiliations

Karolina Rychter
Download PDF Download RIS Download Bibtex

Abstract

Building upon Gasiorowska and Zaleskiewicz's (2021, 2023), we explored how a control threat and attachment style influence social relationship preferences. This experiment aimed to investigate how experiencing a control threat affects individuals with secure, anxious, and avoidant attachment patterns when they can choose between seeking assistance from the market, asking a close person for help, or coping with the situation alone. Participants with different attachment styles were randomly assigned to either the lack of control condition ( n = 290) or the having control condition ( n = 277). Individuals with an anxious attachment were more inclined to choose the market-exchange option and less likely to select the agentic and communal options when faced with a control threat. Meanwhile, those with an avoidant attachment exhibited a higher tendency to choose the agentic option, while their preference for noncontingent help decreased after exposure to the control threat. Surprisingly, secure attachment individuals showed an increased preference for noncontingent help and decreased preferences for market exchange and self-reliance when exposed to the control threat compared to when they had control. These findings suggest that participation in market relationships may meet vital psychological needs and serve as a safeguard against attachment insecurities.
Go to article

Authors and Affiliations

Agata Gasiorowska
1
ORCID: ORCID
Tomasz Zaleskiewicz
1
ORCID: ORCID

  1. SWPS University SWPS University, Wrocław, Poland
Download PDF Download RIS Download Bibtex

Abstract

Studies based on the most common diagnostic categories do not bring conclusive results concerning the overlapping and distinctive features of anxiety and depression, especially in the areas of attentional functioning, structure of affect, and cognitive emotion regulation. However, a new typology has been proposed which treats anxiety and depression as personality types (Fajkowska, 2013). These types – arousal and apprehension anxiety as well as valence and anhedonic depression – are constructed based on two criteria: specific structure and functions (reactive or regulative). The present paper critically examines the empirical evidence related to this approach. The data mostly confirmed the prediction that the similarities and differences in attentional and affective functioning among the anxiety and depression types would be related to their shared and specific structural and functional characteristics. The new typology turned out to be suitable for integrating the existing research findings by relating them to the structure and functions of anxiety and depression. As a result, it is useful in explaining some of the inconsistencies in literature, as it allows to identify the overlapping and distinctive features of the anxiety and depression types. It also helps to understand the mechanisms contributing to the development and maintenance of anxiety and depression, which might be useful in diagnosis and treatment. However, even though Fajkowska’s approach is an important contribution to the understanding of anxiety and depression, it is not exhaustive. Its limitations are discussed, along with proposed modifications of the theory, as well as further research directions.

Go to article

Authors and Affiliations

Ewa Domaradzka
Download PDF Download RIS Download Bibtex

Abstract

Starting in the early years of education, math anxiety is negatively related to mathematic outcomes, therefore there is a need for its adequate measurement in young children. This study presents the psychometric properties of the modified Abbreviated Math Anxiety Scale for Elementary Children (mAMAS-E) for first- to third-grade children based on mAMAS. The validity of mAMAS-E was determined by a series of tests. The analysis confirmed its two-factor structure (Testing and Learning), positive relationships between mAMAS-E and math, general, and test anxiety, and a negative relationship with mathematical achievement. Children with a high level of math self-esteem and math self-confidence (but not Polish language self-esteem and self-confidence) have lower math anxiety in comparison to those with a moderate level. The results also indicate that girls have a higher level of math anxiety than boys. The validity and internal consistency of mAMAS-E are satisfactory; therefore, mAMAS-E may be a recommendable questionnaire for measuring math anxiety in young children.

Go to article

Authors and Affiliations

Monika Szczygieł
ORCID: ORCID
Download PDF Download RIS Download Bibtex

Abstract

Temporomandibular disorders (TMD) encompass a diverse group of abnormalities in the functioning of the masticatory muscles, temporomandibular joints, and surrounding structures within the facial skull. One of the important etiological factors, contributing to the development of TMD are psychoemotional disorders, which include: depression, dysthymia, personality disorders, panic attacks as well as states and anxiety neuroses.
The aim of the study was to carry out a retrospective evaluation of the occurrence of psychoemotional disorders reported in the application form of medical interview of patients treated at the Consulting Room of Temporomandibular Disorders in Dental Institute (University Dental Clinic) for TMD.
The research material consisted of a subjective survey according to the protocol of the RDC/TMD questionnaire, axis II, of the 360 patients (224 women, 136 men), aged 19 to 43 who came to the University Dental Clinic in Krakow due to TMD management.
Result: The results of the conducted studies indicate the common occurrence of emotional disorders in the group of patients treated for TMD, both in the group of muscular and joint form of dysfunctions. The most often the patients selected: loss of sexual interest or pleasure, crying easily, feeling lonely, indifference to every-thing and feeling of worthlessness. These aspects show a significant influence of emotional factors on TMD.
Conclusion: The results of the conducted research indicate a significant frequencies of psychological and emotional disturbances reported in a survey among patients with TMD. Key
Go to article

Authors and Affiliations

Małgorzata Pihut
1
Magdalena Krasińska-Mazur
1
Joanna Biegańska-Banaś
2
Andrzej Gala
1

  1. Consulting Room of Temporomandibular, Disorders, Dental Institute, Prosthodontics Department, Jagiellonian University Medical College, Kraków, Poland
  2. Institute of Nursing and Midwifery, Department of Health Psychology, Jagiellonian University Medical College, Kraków, Poland
Download PDF Download RIS Download Bibtex

Abstract

We explore how conspiracy beliefs change the effectiveness of gain- vs. loss-framed messages in promoting health-protective behavior. We focused on various recommended COVID-19 protective measures, not only vaccinations but also other preventive (like wearing masks) and detection behaviors (like testing). Our results indicate that conspiracy beliefs moderate the effectiveness of gain vs. loss framing. When participants endorse conspiracy worldviews above the average level, the gain frame may be more effective than the loss frame. In other words, in the loss frame condition, conspiracy beliefs negatively and significantly predicted attitudes toward the behavior recommended. However, in the case of the gain frame, the relationship between conspiracist views and attitudes toward promoted behavior was weaker or even nonsignificant. We also found, although only in the case of one behavior, that when participants' eagerness to look for conspiracies almost does not exist, the loss frame may be a better option than the gain frame. Finally, neither of these effects was mediated by emotional reactance or anxiety.
Go to article

Authors and Affiliations

Wojciech Cwalina
1
ORCID: ORCID
Paweł Koniak
1
ORCID: ORCID

  1. Maria Curie-Skłodowska University, Lublin, Poland
Download PDF Download RIS Download Bibtex

Abstract

This is an analysis of the literary expressions of fear in the poetry of Kazimierz Przerwa-Tetmajer. Intense fear is not only one of the dominant emotions depicted in his work but it also underpins his poetic technique. The article examines the manner in which he communicates the somatic experience of dread, his use of bodily metaphors in descriptions of nature, his poetic landscapes of fear, the records of his nightmares, the modernist phobophobia as well the sources of the high anxiety (to do with the mysteries of life, death and man’s inner life) that dogged him at all times. In its readings the article draws on the studies in affect theory, somatopoetics and psychoanalytic criticism.
Go to article

Authors and Affiliations

Lidia Kamińska
1
ORCID: ORCID

  1. Szkoła Doktorska Nauk Humanistycznych, Wydział Polonistyki UJ

This page uses 'cookies'. Learn more