Search results

Filters

  • Journals
  • Authors
  • Keywords
  • Date
  • Type

Search results

Number of results: 20
items per page: 25 50 75
Sort by:
Download PDF Download RIS Download Bibtex

Abstract

While personality is strongly related to experienced emotions, few studies examined the role of personality traits on affective forecasting. In the present study, we investigated the relationships between extraversion and neuroticism personality traits and affective predictions about academic performance. Participants were asked to predict their emotional reactions two months before they will get their results for one important exam. At the same time, personality was assessed with the Big Five Inventory. All the participants were contacted by a text message eight hours after that the results were available, and they were requested to rate their experienced affective state. Results show moderate negative correlations between neuroticism and both predicted and experienced feelings, and that extraversion exhibits a weak positive correlation with predicted feelings, but not with experienced feelings. Taken together, these findings confirm that extraversion and neuroticism shape emotional forecasts, and suggest that affective forecasting interventions based on personality could probably enhance their efficiencies.

Go to article

Authors and Affiliations

Michel Hansenne
Virginie Christophe
Download PDF Download RIS Download Bibtex

Abstract

Cheerleading is a new sport, practiced in 110 nations; since 2016 enjoys provisional Olympic status. Its leaders claim that it is a “happy” sport, but research on its psychological effects is lacking. In this field-study we examined core-affect, positive-affect, and negative-affect in 65 cheerleaders before, during, after, and one-hour after a cheerleading training. Core-affect was more positive during and immediately after training, but it tapered off one hour following the training when feeling states were still more positive than at baseline. Negative-affect declined linearly from baseline to one-hour following training when it became significantly lower than its previous values. Positive-affect showed quadratic dynamics, in parallel with arousal, being higher during and immediately after training than during baseline, or one-hour after training. These results demonstrate for the first time that cheerleading is a “happy” sport, which apart from the skill-development also yields positive psychological emotions both during and after training.

Go to article

Authors and Affiliations

Rita Kovácsik
Attila Szabo
Download PDF Download RIS Download Bibtex

Abstract

The study of inner speech is appropriate to carry out in the plane of interaction of various scientific studies. This approach allows us to analyze the specifics of functioning of inner speech at the level of artistic discourse. Such phenomenon as inner speech presents not only the protagonist's outlook, their emotional state or social aspects, but also demonstrates, under the influence of extraordinary factors, the intensity of affect expression in the addresser's speech activity. Inner speech in an emotive situation is marked by peculiar characteristics, which indicates its unique multidimensional essence.

Go to article

Authors and Affiliations

Nataliia Kravchenko
Download PDF Download RIS Download Bibtex

Abstract

This study presents the consequences of incidental affect when performing a letter search in a complex visual field. Participants were exposed to two superficially unrelated tasks in succession. First, they had to read and remember as much as possible from among 135 emotional words chosen to enable manipulation of two affective factors, valence and origin of emotional state, in a 3x3 factorial design with alignment of other variables, such as arousal, concreteness, frequency of appearance and length. The second task was based on a visual search paradigm. Participants viewed a display of six letters and responded if at least one of two target letters was present. Analysis of reaction latencies for correct responses showed that valence of the words read in the first task had no effect on visual search effectiveness. The origin of the affective state elicited by the words in the first task did influence response latencies: latencies were longer when the first task involved reading words eliciting emotions of automatic origin rather than words eliciting emotions of reflective origin. This study provides further evidence that valence effects found in earlier studies could be accounted for by other dimensions, especially origin of emotional state.
Go to article

Authors and Affiliations

Kamil Imbir
Download PDF Download RIS Download Bibtex

Abstract

In analyzing selected aspects of the debate over offending religious feelings, the author discusses Saby Mahmood’s argument that religiousness in public discourses of the Western world is basically perceived as a speculative phenomenon concerning the sphere of abstract beliefs. It is assumed therefore that the harm that can be produced by the publication of a blasphemous illustration is lesser and less palpable than in the case of hate speech directed toward a race or sexual orientation. The author’s analysis, which is undertaken from a Durkheim perspective, shows that, for example, the caricaturized presentation of a religious symbol constitutes not so much an act of undermining the abstract image as—in the affective perspective of the religious—an act violating the sense of ontological security of a given moral community which that symbol represents. At the same time, the Durkheim perspective facilitates an understanding of why religious symbolic resources can be ambivalently used in processes of legitimating social actions, beginning with constructive forms of civil public religions to extreme fundamentalist movements making use of violence and the discourses of political extremism.

Go to article

Authors and Affiliations

Rafał Smoczyński
Download PDF Download RIS Download Bibtex

Abstract

Four types of self-standards (ideal, ought, undesired, and forbidden selves) were analyzed in the context of self-assessed health of older adults. We focused on the relationships between self-discrepancies (perceived actualization of self-standards) and affect, as well as the content of self-descriptions of standards. Participants (116 Polish older adults) completed: Self Standards’ Measure (SSM), PANAS-X and 7 items from the WHOQOL-BREF. First, we found that self-assessed health moderates the effects of self-discrepancies on affect. The ideal and ought self-discrepancies predicted affect when health was assessed as good. Conversely, the undesired and forbidden self-discrepancies predicted affect when health was assessed as poor. Second, health-related content was more typical for the ideal than for the ought standards. Third, older adults who assessed their health better had fewer health-related standards. The results are discussed with reference to control theory of approach and avoidance.

Go to article

Authors and Affiliations

Wacław Bąk
Donat N. Dutkiewicz
Jan Kutnik
ORCID: ORCID
Download PDF Download RIS Download Bibtex

Abstract

According to the social psychological literature, egoistic relative deprivation impairs well-being but has at most little impact on political protest and engagement. We considered this view incomplete and over-simplifying. It was predicted that egoistic relative deprivation itself may impair democracy by increasing support for populism, and that the postulated relationship will be mediated by dysphoric affect, political alienation, and political paranoia. The empirical basis of the article were three studies carried out on nationwide random-quota samples of adult Poles: in 2002 (N = 1500), in 2010 (N = 800), and in 2017 (N = 2000). It was found that support for populism systematically depends directly on dysphoric affect and political paranoia, which are strengthened by egoistic relative deprivation. In 2017, with populist political groupings in power in Poland, the role of political alienation turned out to be ambiguous. Political alienation slightly strengthened political paranoia but directly lowered support for populism. In conclusion it was noted that in Poland's history and in the contemporary Polish mentality there is a great potential for negative affect, pessimism and bitterness. Relative deprivation experienced today successfully maintains this potential.
Go to article

Bibliography

Abrams, D., Grant, P., R., (2012). Testing the social identity relative deprivation (SIRD) model of social change: The political rise of Scottish nationalism. British Journal of Social Psychology, 51, 674–689. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8309.2011.02032.x
Beck, A., T., Ward, C., H., Mendelson, M., Mock, J., Erbaugh, J., (1961). An inventory for measuring depression. Archives of General Psychiatry, 4, 561-571.
Bernburg, J., G., Thorlindsson, T., Sigfusdottir, I., D., (2009). Relative deprivation and adolescent outcomes in Iceland: A multilevel test. Social Forces, 87(3), 1223-1250. https://doi.org/10.1353/sof.0.0177
Brader, T., Marcus, G., E., (2013). Emotion and political psychology. In: Huddy, L., Sears, D., O., Levy, J., S., (Eds.). The Oxford handbook of political psychology (pp. 165-204). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
Callan, M., J., Kim, H., Matthews, W., J., (2015). Predicting self-rated mental and physical health: The contributions of subjective socio-economic status and personal relative deprivation. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 1415. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01415
Canovan, M., (2002). Populism. New York, NY: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich. Cantril, H., (1965). The pattern of human concerns. New Brunswick, NJ: Rutgers University Press.
Corning, A., F., (2000). Assessing perceived social inequity: A relative deprivation framework. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78(3), 463-477. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.78.3.463
Crosby, F., (1976). A model of egoistical relative deprivation. Psychological Review, 83(2), 85-113. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033- 295x.83.2.85
Czapiński, J., Panek, T., (red.) (2009). Diagnoza Społeczna 2009. Warunki i jakość życia Polaków. http://www.diagnoza.com/pliki/ raporty/Diagnoza_raport_2009.pdf (13.07.2020).
Czapiński, J., Panek, T., (red.) (2015). Diagnoza Społeczna 2015. Warunki i jakość życia Polaków. http://www.diagnoza.com/pliki/ raporty/Diagnoza_raport_2015.pdf (13.07.2020).
Dambrun, M., Taylor, D., M., McDonald, D., A., Crush, J., Meot, A., (2006). The relative deprivation–gratification continuum and the attitudes of South Africans toward immigrants: A test of the V-curve hypothesis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 91(6), 1032–1044. https://doi.org/10.1037/t33410-000
Davies, N., (1981). God’s playground. A history of Poland. Volume II: 1795 to the present. Oxford, U.K.: Oxford University Press. https:// doi.org/10.2307/1865504
de la Rey, C., Raju, P., (1996). Group relative deprivation: Cognitive versus affective components and protest orientation among Indian South Africans. Journal of Social Psychology, 136(5), 579-588.
de la Sablonnière, R., Auger, E., Sadykova, N., Taylor, D., M., (2010). When the ‘‘We’’ impacts how ‘‘I’’ feel about myself. Effect of temporal collective relative deprivation on personal well-being in the context of dramatic social change in Kyrgyzstan. European Psychologist, 15(4), 271–282. https://doi.org/10.1027/1016-9040/ a000062
Eibner, C., Evans, W., N., (2005). Relative deprivation, poor health habits, and mortality. The Journal of Human Resources, XI (3), 591-620. https://doi.org/10.3368/jhr.xl.3.591
European Social Survey 2012 (2012). http://www.europeansocialsurvey. org/download.html?file=ESS6e02_1andy=2012 (13.07.2020).
European Social Survey 2014 (2014). http://www.europeansocialsurvey. org/download.html?file=ESS7e01andy=2014 (13.07.2020).
Feather, N., T., (2015). Analyzing relative deprivation in relation to deservingness, entitlement and resentment. Social Justice Research, 28, 7–26. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11211-015-0235-9
Foster, M., D., Matheson, K., (1995). Double relative deprivation: Combining the personal and the political. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 21(11), 1167-1177. https://doi.org/10.1177/ 01461672952111005
Gerber, E., R., (1999). The populist paradox. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.
Grant, P., R., Abrams, D., Robertson, D., W., Garay, J., (2015). Predicting protests by disadvantaged skilled immigrants: A test of an integrated social identity, relative deprivation, collective efficacy (SIRDE) Model. Social Justice Research, 28, 76–101. https://doi.org/ 10.1007/s11211-014-0229-z
Greven, T., (2016). The rise of right-wing populism in Europe and the United States: A comparative perspective. Berlin: Friedrich-Ebert-Stiftung.
Guiso, L., Herrera, H., Morelli, M., Sonno, T., (2017). Demand and supply of populism. EIEF Working Paper 3/2017. Rome: Einaudi Institute for Economics and Finance. https://doi.org/10.14612/ guiso_herrera_morelli_sonno_1-2_2017
Held, J., (Ed.) (1966). Populism in Eastern Europe: Racism, nationalism and society. New York, NY: Boulder: East European Monographs. https://doi.org/10.2307/310213
Hooghe, M., Reeskens, T., (2007). Are cross-national surveys the best way to study the extreme-right vote in Europe? Patterns of Prejudice, 41 (2), 177-195. https://doi.org/10.1080/0031322 0701265544
Inglehart, R., F., and Norris, P., (2016). Trump, Brexit, and the rise of populism: Economic have-nots and cultural backlash. Faculty Research Working Paper Series. Cambridge, MA: Harvard Kennedy School. https://doi.org/10.2139/ssrn.2818659
Ionescu, G., Gellner, E., (Eds.) (1970). Populism: Its meaning and national characteristic. London and Sydney: Weidenfeld and Nicolson. https://doi.org/10.2307/3102910
Korzeniowski, K., (1994). Political alienation in Poland in days of systemic transformation. Polish Psychological Bulletin, 25(3), 187-200.
Korzeniowski, K., (2010). Polska paranoja polityczna. Źródła, mechanizmy i konsekwencje spiskowego myślenia o polityce. [ Polish political paranoia. Determinants, mechanisms and consequences of conspir-atorial thinking about politics] Warszawa: Wydawnictwo Instytutu Psychologii PAN.
Krasodomski-Jones, A., (2019). Suspicious minds. Conspiracy theories in the age of populism. Brussels: Wilfried Martens Centre for European Studies.
Leach, C., W., Iyer, A., Pedersen, A., (2007). Angry opposition to government redress: When the structurally advantaged perceive themselves as relatively deprived. British Journal of Social Psychology, (2007), 46, 191–204. https://doi.org/10.1348/014466606x 99360
Lewicka, M., (2004). Psychologiczne mechanizmy ludzkiej roszczeniowości (raport z realizacji grantu KBN nr 5 H01F 022 21). Warszawa: Wydział Psychologii Uniwersytetu Warszawskiego.
MacKinnon, D., P., Krull, J., M., Lockwood C., M., (2000). Equivalence of the mediation, confounding and suppression effect. Prevention Science, 1(4), 173–181.
Marchlewska, M., Castellanos, K., A., Lewczuk, K., Kofta, M., Cichocka, A., (2018). My way or the highway: High narcissism and low self- esteem predict decreased support for democracy. British Journal of Social Psychology, https://doi.org/10.1111/bjso.12290
Meny, Y., Surel, Y., (Eds.). (2002). Democracies and the populist challenge. London, UK: Pelgrave Macmillan. https://doi.org/ 10.1057/9781403920072
Miller, D., A., Cronin, T., Garcia, A,. L., Branscombe, N., R., (2009). The relative impact of anger and efficacy on collective action is affected by feelings of fear. Group Processes and Intergroup Relations, 12, 445–462. https://doi.org/10.1177/1368430209105046
Miller, D., T., Ross, M., (1975). Self-serving biases in the attributions of causality: Fact or fiction? Psychological Bulletin, 82, 213-225. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0076486
Osborne, D., Huo, Y., J., Smith, H., J., (2015).Organizational respect dampens the impact of group-based relative deprivation on will-ingness to protest pay cuts. British Journal of Social Psychology, 54, 159–175. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjso.12069
Osborne, D., Sibley, C., G., (2015). Opposing paths to ideology: Group- based relative deprivation predicts conservatism through warmth toward ingroup and outgroup members. Social Justice Research, 28, 27–51. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11211-014-0227-1
Paulhus, D., L., Robins, R., W., Trzesniewski, K., H., Tracy, J., L., (2004). Two replicable suppressor situations in personality research. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 39(2), 303–328. https://doi.org/ 10.1207/s15327906mbr3902_7
Pettigrew, T., F., Philipps, O., C., Philipps, U., W., Meertens, R., W., van Dick, R., Zick, A., (2008). Relative deprivation and intergroup prejudice. Journal of Social Issues, 64(2), 385-401.
Pipes, D., (1997). Conspiracy. How the paranoid style flourishes and where it comes from. New York, NY: The Free Press. https://doi.org/10.2307/20048795
Robins, R., S., Post, (1997). Political paranoia. The psychopolitics of hatred. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
Runciman, W., G., (1966). Relative deprivation and social justice. London, UK: Routledge Kegan Paul.
Schenkkan, N., (2017). Nations in transit 2017: The false promise of populism. Washington, DC: Freedom House.
Schenkkan, N., (2018). Nations in transit 2018: Confronting Illiberalism. Washington, D.C.: Freedom House.
Schmitt, M., Maes, J., (2002). Stereotypic ingroup bias as self-defense against relative deprivation: evidence from a longitudinal study of the German unification process. European Journal of Social Psychology, 32, 309–326. https://doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.91
Schmitt, M., Maes, J., Widaman, K., (2010). Longitudinal effects of egoistic and fraternal relative deprivation on well-being and protest. International Journal of Psychology, 45(2), 122–130. https://doi.org/ 10.1080/00207590903165067
Smith, H., J., Pettigrew, T., F., (2014). Interpretation of inequality: A model of the relative deprivation experience. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 8/12, 755-765. https://doi.org/ 10.1111/spc3.12151
Smith, H., J., Pettigrew, T., F., Pippin, G., M., Bialosiewicz, S., (2012). Relative deprivation: A theoretical and meta-analytic review. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 16(3) 203 –232.
Smith, H., J., Ortiz, D., J., (2002). Is it just me?: The different consequences of personal and group relative deprivation. In: Walker, I., Smith, H., J., (Eds.) Relative deprivation: Specification, development, and integration (pp. 91-115). New York, NY,: Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/cbo9780511527753.005
Solomon, R., L., (1980). The opponent-process theory of acquired motivation: The costs of pleasure and the beneÞts of pain. American Psychologist, 35(8), 691–712. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-009- 8225-3_18
Stouffer, S., A., Suchman, E., A., DeVinney, L., C., Star, S., A., Williams, R., M., Jr., (1949). The American soldier: Vol. 1. Adjustment during army life. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.
Turley, R., N., L., (2002). Is relative deprivation beneficial? The effects of richer and poorer neighbors on children’s outcomes. Journal of Community Psychology, 30(6), 671–686. https://doi.org/10.1002/ jcop.10033
Walker, I., Pettigrew, T., F., (1984). Relative deprivation theory: An overview and conceptual critique. British Journal of Social Psychology, 23(4), 301–310. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8309.1984. tb00645.x
Wickham, S., Shryane, N., Lyons, M., Dickins, T., Bentall, R., (2014). Why does relative deprivation affect mental health? The role of justice, trust and social rank in psychological wellbeing and paranoid ideation. Journal of Public Mental Health, 13(2), 114-126. https:// doi.org/10.1108/jpmh-06-2013-0049
Zagefka, H., Binder, J., Brown, R., Hancock, L., (2013).Who is to blame? The relationship between ingroup identification and relative deprivation is moderated by ingroup attributions. Social Psychology, 44(6), 398–407. https://doi.org/10.1027/1864-9335/a000153
Zagefka, H., Pehrson, S., Mole, R., C., Chan, E., (2010). The effect of essentialism in settings of historic intergroup atrocities. European Journal of Social Psychology, 40(5), 718–732. https://doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.639
Go to article

Authors and Affiliations

Krzysztof Korzeniowski
1

  1. Polish Academy of Sciences
Download PDF Download RIS Download Bibtex

Abstract

The paper describes a research on assessing the quality of edges resulting from the interaction of laser pulses with a material of rigid and flexible printed circuits. A modern Nd:YVO4 crystal diode-pumped solid-state laser generating a 532 nm wavelength radiation with a nanosecond pulse time was used for the research. Influence of laser parameters such as beam power and pulse repetition frequency on a heat affected zone and carbonization was investigated. Quality and morphology of laser-cut substrates were analyzed by optical microscopy. High quality laser cutting of printed circuit board substrates was obtained without delamination and surface damage, with a minimal carbonization and heat affected zone. The developed process was implemented on the printed circuit assembly line.

Go to article

Authors and Affiliations

P. Ciszewski
M. Sochacki
Download PDF Download RIS Download Bibtex

Abstract

Given the whole spectrum of doubts and controversies that arise in discussions about laws affecting historical memory (and their subcategory of memory laws), the question of assessing them in the context of international standards of human rights protection – and in particular the European system of human rights protection – is often overlooked. Thus this article focuses on the implications and conditions for introducing memory laws in light of international human rights standards using selected examples of various types of recently-adopted Polish memory laws as case studies. The authors begin with a brief description of the phenomenon of memory laws and the most significant threats that they pose to the protection of international human rights standards. The following sections analyse selected Polish laws affecting historical memory vis-à-vis these standards. The analysis covers non-binding declaratory laws affecting historical memory, and acts that include criminal law sanctions. The article attempts to sketch the circumstances linking laws affecting historical memory with the human rights protection standards, including those entailed both in binding treaties and other instruments of international law.

Go to article

Authors and Affiliations

Aleksandra Gliszczyńska-Grabias
ORCID: ORCID
Grażyna Baranowska
ORCID: ORCID
Anna Wójcik
Download PDF Download RIS Download Bibtex

Abstract

Studies based on the most common diagnostic categories do not bring conclusive results concerning the overlapping and distinctive features of anxiety and depression, especially in the areas of attentional functioning, structure of affect, and cognitive emotion regulation. However, a new typology has been proposed which treats anxiety and depression as personality types (Fajkowska, 2013). These types – arousal and apprehension anxiety as well as valence and anhedonic depression – are constructed based on two criteria: specific structure and functions (reactive or regulative). The present paper critically examines the empirical evidence related to this approach. The data mostly confirmed the prediction that the similarities and differences in attentional and affective functioning among the anxiety and depression types would be related to their shared and specific structural and functional characteristics. The new typology turned out to be suitable for integrating the existing research findings by relating them to the structure and functions of anxiety and depression. As a result, it is useful in explaining some of the inconsistencies in literature, as it allows to identify the overlapping and distinctive features of the anxiety and depression types. It also helps to understand the mechanisms contributing to the development and maintenance of anxiety and depression, which might be useful in diagnosis and treatment. However, even though Fajkowska’s approach is an important contribution to the understanding of anxiety and depression, it is not exhaustive. Its limitations are discussed, along with proposed modifications of the theory, as well as further research directions.

Go to article

Authors and Affiliations

Ewa Domaradzka
Download PDF Download RIS Download Bibtex

Abstract

The display of affection in romantic relationships and its concomitants still require more scientific attention. Despite some studies addressing the topic of affection display, the literature does not provide a psychometrically reliable self-descriptive tool to measure this construct. Therefore, we conducted three studies among Polish adults to develop and validate a psychological tool for comprehensively identifying and measuring the display of emotional affection. Study 1 ( N = 894) aimed to develop and validate the Public and Private Romantic Display of Affection Scale (PPRDAS). It proved to be a valid psychological scale, as the theoretically assumed structure was supported by the results of the empirical analysis. Study 2 ( N = 343) confirmed the convergence validity of the PPRDAS using items of emotional expression from the Dyadic Adjustment Scale (Spanier, 1989). In Study 3 ( N = 204 couples), we further verified the external validity of the PPRDAS using an assessment of affection displayed by one's partner in the relationship. Individuals’ self-estimates of their private and public displays of affection were confirmed by their romantic partners. In all studies, display of feelings was positively correlated with sexual and relationship satisfaction. Negative correlations with age and the duration of the romantic relationship were also observed.
Go to article

Authors and Affiliations

Dagna Joanna Kocur
1
ORCID: ORCID
Monika Prusik
2
ORCID: ORCID
Karolina Konopka
3

  1. University of Silesia, Katowice, Poland
  2. The University of Warsaw, Warsaw, Poland
  3. The Maria Grzegorzewska University, Warsaw, Poland
Download PDF Download RIS Download Bibtex

Abstract

Non-invasive techniques for the assessment of respiratory disorders have gained increased importance in recent years due to the complexity of conventional methods. In the assessment of respiratory disorders, machine learning may play a very essential role. Respiratory disorders lead to variation in the production of speech as both go hand in hand. Thus, speech analysis can be a useful means for the pre-diagnosis of respiratory disorders. This article aims to develop a machine learning approach to differentiate healthy speech from speech corresponding to different respiratory disorders (affected). Thus, in the present work, a set of 15 relevant and efficient features were extracted from acquired data, and classification was done using different classifiers for healthy and affected speech. To assess the performance of different classifiers, accuracy, specificity (Sp), sensitivity (Se), and area under the receiver operating characteristic curve (AUC) was used by applying both multi-fold cross-validation methods (5-fold and 10-fold) and the holdout method. Out of the studied classifiers, decision tree, support vector machine (SVM), and k-nearest neighbor (KNN) were found more appropriate in providing correct assessment clinically while considering 15 features as well as three significant features (Se > 89%, Sp > 89%, AUC> 82%, and accuracy > 99%). The conclusion was that the proposed classifiers may provide an aid in the simple assessment of respiratory disorders utilising speech parameters with high efficiency. In the future, the proposed approach can be evaluated for the detection of specific respiratory disorders such as asthma, COPD, etc.
Go to article

Authors and Affiliations

Poonam Shrivastava
1
Neeta Tripathi
1
Bikesh Kumar Singh
2
Bhupesh Kumar Dewangan
3

  1. Department of Electronics and Telecommunication, SSTC Bhilai, India
  2. Department of Biomedical Engineering, National Institute of Technology, Raipur, India
  3. Department of Computer Science and Engineering, School of Engineering, OP Jindal University, Raigarh, India
Download PDF Download RIS Download Bibtex

Abstract

The coarse-grained heat-affected zone specimens of X80 pipeline steel were produced by welding thermal simulation under different heat inputs of 10, 30, and 55 kJ/cm to study the effects of heat input on microstructure evolution and corrosion characterization. The corrosion resistance of coarse-grained heat-affected zones was poorer than that of base metal due to less homogenous in the former. For 10 kJ/cm coarse-grained heat-affected zone, the corrosion resistance was poorer than the others due to the more adsorption hydrogen around the needle-like martensite/austenite constituents and greater galvanic driving force between the needle-like martensite/austenite constituents and ferrite. In carbonate/bicarbonate solution, better corrosion resistance for coarse-grained heat-affected zones was obtained when the heat input is 30 kJ/cm, which can be attributed to the severe coarse martensite/austenite constituents for 55 kJ/cm coarse-grained heat-affected zone. In the H2S environment, the better corrosion resistance for coarse-grained heat-affected zone was obtained when the heat input is 55 kJ/cm, which can be attributed to the protective effect of corrosion products. In addition, the high content of M/A constituents for 30 kJ/cm CGHAZ was good for hydrogen adsorption, which was adverse to the corrosion resistance in acid environments.
Go to article

Authors and Affiliations

Xue-Mei Wang
1 2
ORCID: ORCID
Wei Zhao
1 2 3
ORCID: ORCID
Kai Chen
1 2
ORCID: ORCID
Zhen Li
1 2
ORCID: ORCID

  1. Qilu University of Technology (Shandong Academy of Sciences), School of Mechanical & Automotive Engineering, China
  2. Shandong Institute of Mechanical Design and Research, China
  3. School of Materials Science and Engineering, Tianjin University, China
Download PDF Download RIS Download Bibtex

Abstract

This research is focused on the analysis of heat-affected sub-zones in 2 mm thick steel S960MC samples, with the aim of observing and evaluating the mechanical properties after exposure to temperatures corresponding to individual heat-affected sub-zones. Test samples were prepared using a Gleeble 3500 thermo-mechanical simulator. The samples were heated in the range from 550°C to 1350°C and were subsequently quickly cooled. The specimens, together with the base material, were then subjected to tensile testing, impact testing, and micro-hardness measurements in the sample cross-section, as well as evaluation of their microstructure. Fracture surfaces are investigated in samples after impact testing. The heat-affected sub-zones studied indicate high sensitivity to the thermal input of welding. There is a significant decrease in tensile strength and yield strength at temperatures around 550°C.

Go to article

Authors and Affiliations

M. Mičian
ORCID: ORCID
J. Winczek
D. Harmaniak
R. Koňár
M. Gucwa
J. Moravec
Download PDF Download RIS Download Bibtex

Abstract

We examined the role of work-related emotions and personal resources operationalised as psychological capital (PsyCap) in the relationship between events occurring at work and employees’ work engagement. Using affective events theory and broaden-and-build theory as theoretical frameworks, we theorise that the perceived frequency of positive and negative events at work and work engagement is mediated by positive and negative work-related emotions and moderated by PsyCap. The results of path analysis on a sample of US and Polish employees showed that PsyCap moderated the relationship between the perceived frequency of negative work events and negative work-related emotions, however, we also found culture-specific effects of PsyCap. Our study contributes to the human resource development (HRD) literature by providing evidence of the role of personal resources in the event–emotion–engagement process in the workplace. Also, our findings deepen the understanding of HR developers in multinational organisations and provide suggestions on how they can implement PsyCap trainings based on culture-specific work environment.
Go to article

Authors and Affiliations

Aleksandra Penza
1
ORCID: ORCID
Agata Gasiorowska
1
ORCID: ORCID

  1. SWPS University, Wrocław, Poland
Download PDF Download RIS Download Bibtex

Abstract

This article presents a critical interpretation of Barbara Klicka’s Zdrój [ The Spa] (2019) within the framework of the Spatial Turn. The analysis examines the relationship between the subject and the medical spaces with the help of concepts like heterotopia, atopia, psychotopography as well as the author’s own concept of topopathography. The aim is to explore the impact of the designated sanatorium space on the patient’s identity.
Go to article

Authors and Affiliations

Wiktoria Kulak
1
ORCID: ORCID

  1. Wydział Polonistyki UJ
Download PDF Download RIS Download Bibtex

Abstract

The aim of this article is to show how Kazimierz Przerwa-Tetmajer depicts the creative process in his autothematic poems. The readings draw on the interpretative strategies of the theory of affect (primarily the work Ernst van Alphen, Teresa Brennan and Brian Massumi) and somatic criticism (e.g. Anna Łebkowska’s somatopoetics, Marta Smo-lińska’s ‘augmented haptics’). In the great majority of Przerwa-Tetmajer’s poems dealing with the process of poetic creation, we are told that it is triggered by an explosion of the affects or an overpowering divine inspiration, bordering on madness. The article ana-lyzes in turn the affective nature of the creative process and the affective perception of a work of art; the image of the artist as craftsman; and, finally, the image of the phantas-matic woman, a product of the act of creation.
Go to article

Authors and Affiliations

Lidia Kamińska
1
ORCID: ORCID

  1. Szkoła Doktorska Nauk Humanistycznych, Uniwersytet Jagielloński
Download PDF Download RIS Download Bibtex

Abstract

This article deals with literary pathography, i.e. texts which purport to project rage or a mental disorder, and use narrative strategies adopted specifically for that purpose. The analysis is focused on two novels by Aleksandra Zielińska, Przypadek Alicji (Alicja’s Case) and Bura i szał (Bura and Rage) treated as literary representations of the protagonists' mental condition. The literary character of these 'records' is revealed by multiple intertextual tropes and poetic devices that deconstruct the cultural stereotype of female rage. Consequently, Aleksandra Zielińska's novels should be seen as projections of a fractured female subject (un sujet divisé) fixed on her somatic vulnerability, driven by an urge to cry out her affliction, trauma and rage, unease about woman-to-woman relations, and the pressure of erratic affective impulses.

Go to article

Authors and Affiliations

Joanna Szewczyk

This page uses 'cookies'. Learn more